Memory as a Constructive Process: The Parallel Distributed Processing Approach

James L. McClelland In Harold Pinter’s play Old Times, a husband and wife of many years reminisce about the early days of their relationship, while awaiting a visit from the wife’s best friend from that era. In these reminiscences, we learn just how differently two people can remember what were ostensibly the same events, and ostensibly the same people – most notably, themselves and the wife’s best friend. For each, these reminiscences have become embedded in a complex, not fully consistent, and self-serving personal history that does not survive well when juxtaposed against the reminiscences of the other. In an interview at the time of the opening of Old Times, Pinter was asked to comment on his thoughts while writing his play. He said “what fascinates me is the mistiness of memory”(1). Cloud-like, forever changing, memories are clearly not like frozen snapshots taken on a day long ago, pulled out from the back of a drawer for reinspection. Indeed, since the work of Frederick Bartlett in 1932, memory researchers have been keenly aware of the constructive nature of memory (2). Bartlett asked educated people at Cambridge to read, and then later to recall, a story from a native North American culture. Though written in English, the story had an unfamiliar structure and content. The recollections of the participants retained elements from the original story but many details were omitted or transformed in ways that seemed to Bartlett to fit better with the cultural context of the individuals who were recalling them. Repeated attempts at recall by the same individual resulted in gradual fixing of the elements, but into a story sometimes quite different from the original. Such findings led Bartlett and others to view recollection as a process not unlike the activities of an archaeologist faced with the task

[1]  S. Tonegawa,et al.  Fear memories require protein synthesis in the amygdala for reconsolidation after retrieval , 2022 .

[2]  Norton W. Milgram,et al.  Post-activation potentiation in the neocortex. IV. Multiple sessions required for induction of long-term potentiation in the chronic preparation , 1995, Brain Research.

[3]  M. Moser,et al.  Pattern Separation in the Dentate Gyrus and CA3 of the Hippocampus , 2007, Science.

[4]  D. Marr A theory of cerebellar cortex , 1969, The Journal of physiology.

[5]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Hippocampal conjunctive encoding, storage, and recall: Avoiding a trade‐off , 1994, Hippocampus.

[6]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Structure and deterioration of semantic memory: a neuropsychological and computational investigation. , 2004, Psychological review.

[7]  W. Scoville,et al.  LOSS OF RECENT MEMORY AFTER BILATERAL HIPPOCAMPAL LESIONS , 1957, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[8]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. , 1995, Psychological review.

[9]  Jeffrey S Bowers,et al.  On the biological plausibility of grandmother cells: implications for neural network theories in psychology and neuroscience. , 2009, Psychological review.

[10]  E. Capaldi,et al.  The organization of behavior. , 1992, Journal of applied behavior analysis.

[11]  B. Milner Amnesia following operation on the temporal lobes , 1996 .

[12]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Hippocampal synaptic enhancement and information storage within a distributed memory system , 1987, Trends in Neurosciences.

[13]  James L. McClelland,et al.  The parallel distributed processing approach to semantic cognition , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[14]  M. Wilson,et al.  Dentate Gyrus NMDA Receptors Mediate Rapid Pattern Separation in the Hippocampal Network , 2007, Science.

[15]  Alex Martin,et al.  Semantic memory and the brain: structure and processes , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[16]  D Marr,et al.  Simple memory: a theory for archicortex. , 1971, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[17]  Michael McCloskey,et al.  Catastrophic Interference in Connectionist Networks: The Sequential Learning Problem , 1989 .

[18]  L. Squire Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. , 1992, Psychological review.

[19]  F. Bartlett,et al.  Remembering: A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology , 1932 .

[20]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Semantic Cognition: A Parallel Distributed Processing Approach , 2004 .

[21]  L. Squire,et al.  Autobiographical memory and amnesia , 1989 .

[22]  T. Bliss,et al.  Long‐lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path , 1973, The Journal of physiology.

[23]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Comparison of spatial and temporal characteristics of neuronal activity in sequential stages of hippocampal processing. , 1990, Progress in brain research.

[24]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep. , 1994, Science.

[25]  Antonio R. Damasio,et al.  The Brain Binds Entities and Events by Multiregional Activation from Convergence Zones , 1989, Neural Computation.

[26]  D. Amaral,et al.  Memory and the Hippocampus , 1989 .

[27]  Yadin Dudai,et al.  Reconsolidation: the advantage of being refocused , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.