Reversal of the physiological effects of monocular deprivation in the kitten's visual cortex.

1. Twenty‐three kittens were monocularly deprived of vision until the age of 4, 5, 6 or 7 weeks. Their deprived eyes were then opened, and their experienced eyes shut for a further 3‐63 days. After this time physiological recordings were made in the visual cortex, area 17. Three control kittens, monocularly deprived for various periods, showed that at the time of reverse‐suturing, few neurones could be influenced at all from the deprived eye. 2. Following reverse‐suturing, the initially deprived eye regained control of cortical neurones. This switch of cortical ocular dominance was most rapid following reverse‐suturing at the age of 4 weeks. Delaying the age of reverse‐suturing reduced the rate and then the extent of the cortical ocular dominance changes. 3. The cortex of reverse‐sutured kittens is divided into regions of cells dominated by one eye or the other. The relative sizes of these ocular dominance columns changed during reversed deprivation. The columns devoted to the initially deprived eye were very small in animals reverse‐sutured for brief periods, but in animals that underwent longer periods of reversed deprivation, the columns driven by that eye were larger, while those devoted to the initially open eye were smaller. 4. Clear progressions of orientation columns across the cortex were apparent in many of the kittens, but, in contrast to the situation in normal or strabismic kittens, these sequences were disrupted at the borders of eye dominance columns: the cortical representations of orientation and ocular dominance were not independent. 5. Binocular units in these kittens were rather rare, but those that could be found often had dissimilar receptive field properties in the two eyes. Commonly, a cell would have a normal orientation selective receptive field in one eye, and an immature, unselective receptive field in the other. Cells that had orientation selective receptive fields in both eyes often had greatly differing orientation preferences in the two eyes, occasionally by nearly 90 degrees. 6. During the reversal of deprivation effects, the proportion of receptive fields exhibiting mature properties declined in the initially experienced eye, while the proportion increased in the initially deprived eye. Similarly, the average band width of orientation tuning of receptive fields in the initially deprived eye decreased, while that of receptive fields in the initially experienced eye increased. 7. One kitten was reverse‐sutured twice, to demonstrate that cortical ocular dominance may be reversed a second time, even after one reversal of ocular dominance. 8. It is suggested that the sensitive period for cortical binocular development consists of two phases. In the first phase, all cortical neurones may be modified by experience, but the rate at which they may be modified decreases with age. In the second phase, an increasing number of cortical neurones becomes fixed in their properties, while those that remain modifiable are as modifiable as they were at the end of the first phase. 9...

[1]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex , 1962, The Journal of physiology.

[2]  D. Hubel,et al.  RECEPTIVE FIELDS OF CELLS IN STRIATE CORTEX OF VERY YOUNG, VISUALLY INEXPERIENCED KITTENS. , 1963, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  D. Hubel,et al.  EFFECTS OF VISUAL DEPRIVATION ON MORPHOLOGY AND PHYSIOLOGY OF CELLS IN THE CATS LATERAL GENICULATE BODY. , 1963, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  D. Hubel,et al.  SINGLE-CELL RESPONSES IN STRIATE CORTEX OF KITTENS DEPRIVED OF VISION IN ONE EYE. , 1963, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  D. Hubel,et al.  Shape and arrangement of columns in cat's striate cortex , 1963, The Journal of physiology.

[6]  H. Barlow,et al.  Retinal ganglion cells responding selectively to direction and speed of image motion in the rabbit , 1964, The Journal of physiology.

[7]  D. Hubel,et al.  Extent of recovery from the effects of visual deprivation in kittens. , 1965, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  D. Hubel,et al.  Binocular interaction in striate cortex of kittens reared with artificial squint. , 1965, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  D. Hubel,et al.  Comparison of the effects of unilateral and bilateral eye closure on cortical unit responses in kittens. , 1965, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  C. Blakemore,et al.  The neural mechanism of binocular depth discrimination , 1967, The Journal of physiology.

[11]  W. Levick Receptive fields and trigger features of ganglion cells in the visual streak of the rabbit's retina , 1967, The Journal of physiology.

[12]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex , 1968, The Journal of physiology.

[13]  M. Jacobson,et al.  Development of specific neuronal connections. , 1969, Science.

[14]  D. Hubel,et al.  The period of susceptibility to the physiological effects of unilateral eye closure in kittens , 1970, The Journal of physiology.

[15]  G. F. Cooper,et al.  Development of the Brain depends on the Visual Environment , 1970, Nature.

[16]  R. Mark,et al.  The mechanism of selective reinnervation of fish eye muscle. I. Evidence from muscle function during recovery. , 1970, Brain research.

[17]  L. Marotte,et al.  The mechanism of selective reinnervation of fish eye muscle. II. Evidence from electronmicroscopy of nerve endings. , 1970, Brain research.

[18]  H. Barlow,et al.  Lack of specificity of neurones in the visual cortex of young kittens. , 1971, The Journal of physiology.

[19]  P. O. Bishop,et al.  Interaction effects of visual contours on the discharge frequency of simple striate neurones , 1971, The Journal of physiology.

[20]  B. Dreher Hypercomplex cells in the cat's striate cortex. , 1972, Investigative ophthalmology.

[21]  B. Wickelgren-Gordon,et al.  Some effects of visual deprivation on the cat superior colliculus. , 1972, Investigative ophthalmology.

[22]  C. Blakemore,et al.  A second neural mechanism of binocular depth discrimination , 1972, The Journal of physiology.

[23]  D. L. Stewart,et al.  Reversal of structural and functional effects of long-term visual deprivation in cats. , 1972, Experimental neurology.

[24]  D. Hubel,et al.  Laminar and columnar distribution of geniculo‐cortical fibers in the macaque monkey , 1972, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[25]  G. Stent A physiological mechanism for Hebb's postulate of learning. , 1973, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[26]  J. Pettigrew Binocular Neurones which signal Change of Disparity in Area 18 of Cat Visual Cortex , 1973 .

[27]  P. O. Bishop,et al.  Receptive fields of simple cells in the cat striate cortex , 1973, The Journal of physiology.

[28]  H. Hirsch,et al.  Cortical effect of selective visual experience: degeneration or reorganization? , 1973, Brain research.

[29]  D H Hubel,et al.  Autoradiographic demonstration of ocular-dominance columns in the monkey striate cortex by means of transneuronal transport. , 1974, Brain research.

[30]  D. Hubel,et al.  Sequence regularity and geometry of orientation columns in the monkey striate cortex , 1974, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[31]  P. O. Bishop,et al.  Orientation specificity of cells in cat striate cortex. , 1974, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  C. Blakemore,et al.  Reversal of the physiological effects of monocular deprivation in kittens: further evidence for a sensitive period , 1974, The Journal of physiology.

[33]  W. Levick,et al.  Brisk and sluggish concentrically organized ganglion cells in the cat's retina , 1974, The Journal of physiology.

[34]  P. O. Bishop,et al.  Orientation, axis and direction as stimulus parameters for striate cells. , 1974, Vision research.

[35]  L. Palmer,et al.  Visual receptive fields of single striate corical units projecting to the superior colliculus in the cat. , 1974, Brain research.

[36]  D. Hubel,et al.  Ordered arrangement of orientation columns in monkeys lacking visual experience , 1974, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[37]  J. Pettigrew,et al.  The effect of visual experience on the development of stimulus specificity by kitten cortical neurones , 1974, The Journal of physiology.

[38]  C. Blakemore,et al.  Functional reinnervation in kitten visual cortex , 1974, Nature.

[39]  C. Blakemore,et al.  Evidence for a loss of afferent axons in the visual cortex of monocularly deprived cats , 1975, Neuroscience Letters.

[40]  D. Hubel,et al.  The pattern of ocular dominance columns in macaque visual cortex revealed by a reduced silver stain , 1975, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[41]  C. Blakemore,et al.  Innate and environmental factors in the development of the kitten's visual cortex. , 1975, The Journal of physiology.

[42]  B. Gordon,et al.  Effects of extraocular muscle section on receptive fields in cat superior colliculus , 1975, Vision Research.

[43]  B. Cragg,et al.  The development of synapses in the visual system of the cat , 1975, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[44]  D. Hubel,et al.  Functional architecture of area 17 in normal and monocularly deprived macaque monkeys. , 1976, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.

[45]  C. Blakemore,et al.  Synaptic competition in the kitten's visual cortex. , 1976, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.

[46]  J. Movshon,et al.  Reversal of the morphological effects of monocular deprivation in the kittens's lateral geniculate nucleus. , 1976, The Journal of physiology.

[47]  J. Movshon Reversal of the behavioural effects of monocular deprivation in the kitten. , 1976, The Journal of physiology.

[48]  Ryosaku Otsuka,et al.  Über Aufbau und Gliederung der corticalen Sehsphäre bei der Katze , 2004, Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten.