Striatal activation during acquisition of a cognitive skill.

The striatum is thought to play an essential role in the acquisition of a wide range of motor, perceptual, and cognitive skills, but neuroimaging has not yet demonstrated striatal activation during nonmotor skill learning. Functional magnetic resonance imaging was performed while participants learned probabilistic classification, a cognitive task known to rely on procedural memory early in learning and declarative memory later in learning. Multiple brain regions were active during probabilistic classification compared with a perceptual-motor control task, including bilateral frontal cortices, occipital cortex, and the right caudate nucleus in the striatum. The left hippocampus was less active bilaterally during probabilistic classification than during the control task, and the time course of this hippocampal deactivation paralleled the expected involvement of medial temporal structures based on behavioral studies of amnesic patients. Findings provide initial evidence for the role of frontostriatal systems in normal cognitive skill learning.

[1]  G. Schwartz,et al.  Consciousness and Self-Regulation , 1976 .

[2]  L. Squire,et al.  Preserved learning and retention of pattern-analyzing skill in amnesia: dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. , 1980, Science.

[3]  J. Becker,et al.  Dissociations between skill learning and verbal recognition in amnesia and dementia. , 1984, Archives of neurology.

[4]  Donald A. Norman,et al.  Attention to Action , 1986 .

[5]  G. Logan Toward an instance theory of automatization. , 1988 .

[6]  J. Saint-Cyr,et al.  Procedural learning and neostriatal dysfunction in man. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[7]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  Hippocampal system dysfunction and odor discrimination learning in rats: impairment or facilitation depending on representational demands. , 1988, Behavioral neuroscience.

[8]  M. Packard,et al.  Differential effects of fornix and caudate nucleus lesions on two radial maze tasks: evidence for multiple memory systems , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[9]  D. Salmon,et al.  Neuropsychological evidence for multiple implicit memory systems: a comparison of Alzheimer's, Huntington's, and Parkinson's disease patients , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[10]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[11]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The Relationship between Global and Local Changes in PET Scans , 1990, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[12]  L. Squire,et al.  The Neuropsychology of Memory , 1990 .

[13]  F M Miezin,et al.  Activation of the hippocampus in normal humans: a functional anatomical study of memory. , 1992, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[14]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Functional anatomy of human procedural learning determined with regional cerebral blood flow and PET , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[15]  L. Squire Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. , 1992, Psychological review.

[16]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Rapid Automated Algorithm for Aligning and Reslicing PET Images , 1992, Journal of computer assisted tomography.

[17]  D. Willingham,et al.  Evidence for dissociable motor skills in Huntington’s disease patients , 1993, Psychobiology.

[18]  T. Robbins,et al.  Contrasting mechanisms of impaired attentional set-shifting in patients with frontal lobe damage or Parkinson's disease. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[19]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  Memory, amnesia, and the hippocampal system , 1993 .

[20]  M. Gluck,et al.  Probabilistic classification learning in amnesia. , 1994, Learning & memory.

[21]  P F Liddle,et al.  The role of the left prefrontal cortex in verbal processing: semantic processing or willed action? , 1994, Neuroreport.

[22]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Analysis of functional MRI time‐series , 1994, Human Brain Mapping.

[23]  Niels Birbaumer,et al.  Memory and Skill Acquisition in Parkinson's Disease and Frontal Lobe Dysfunction , 1995, Cortex.

[24]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Functional Mapping of Sequence Learning in Normal Humans , 1995, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[25]  F. Miezin,et al.  Functional anatomical studies of explicit and implicit memory retrieval tasks , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[26]  Adrian M. Owen,et al.  Dopamine-Dependent Frontostriatal Planning Deficits in Early Parkinson's Disease , 1995 .

[27]  S P Wise,et al.  Distributed modular architectures linking basal ganglia, cerebellum, and cerebral cortex: their role in planning and controlling action. , 1995, Cerebral cortex.

[28]  Joel L. Davis,et al.  Adaptive Critics and the Basal Ganglia , 1995 .

[29]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Regional cerebral activity associated with the incidental processing of pseudo‐words , 1995 .

[30]  J. Gabrieli Contribution of the basal ganglia to skill learning and working memory in humans , 1995 .

[31]  L R Squire,et al.  Dissociable properties of memory systems: differences in the flexibility of declarative and nondeclarative knowledge. , 1996, Behavioral neuroscience.

[32]  Jane S. Paulsen,et al.  Dissociations within nondeclarative memory in Huntington's disease. , 1996 .

[33]  Jennifer A. Mangels,et al.  A Neostriatal Habit Learning System in Humans , 1996, Science.

[34]  R. Dolan,et al.  Active representation of shape and spatial location in man. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[35]  R. Dolan,et al.  Neural systems engaged by planning: a PET study of the Tower of London task , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  C Caltagirone,et al.  Implicit memory in parkinsonian patients: evidence for deficient skill learning. , 1996, European neurology.

[37]  N. Alpert,et al.  Conscious recollection and the human hippocampal formation: evidence from positron emission tomography. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[38]  J. B. Demb,et al.  Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of Semantic Memory Processes in the Frontal Lobes , 1996 .

[39]  J. Doyon,et al.  Role of the Striatum, Cerebellum, and Frontal Lobes in the Learning of a Visuomotor Sequence , 1997, Brain and Cognition.

[40]  J. Desmond,et al.  Neural Substrates of Fluid Reasoning: An fMRI Study of Neocortical Activation during Performance of the Raven's Progressive Matrices Test , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.

[41]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  A Parametric Study of Prefrontal Cortex Involvement in Human Working Memory , 1996, NeuroImage.

[42]  G H Glover,et al.  Separate neural bases of two fundamental memory processes in the human medial temporal lobe. , 1997, Science.

[43]  J. Binder,et al.  Functional MRI evidence for subcortical participation in conceptual reasoning skills , 1997, Neuroreport.

[44]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Society for Neuroscience Abstracts , 1997 .

[45]  S. Rauch,et al.  Striatal recruitment during an implicit sequence learning task as measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging , 1997, Human brain mapping.

[46]  A. Owen The Functional Organization of Working Memory Processes Within Human Lateral Frontal Cortex: The Contribution of Functional Neuroimaging , 1997, The European journal of neuroscience.

[47]  J. Feigenbaum,et al.  Planning Ability After Frontal and Temporal Lobe Lesions in Humans: The Effects of Selection Equivocation and Working Memory Load , 1997 .

[48]  P. Roland,et al.  Bilateral activation of fronto-parietal networks by incrementing demand in a working memory task. , 1997, Cerebral cortex.

[49]  Hippocampal glycogen metabolism, EEG, and behavior. , 1997 .

[50]  J. Desmond,et al.  Prefrontal cortex and recognition memory. Functional-MRI evidence for context-dependent retrieval processes. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[51]  G. Glover,et al.  Self‐navigated spiral fMRI: Interleaved versus single‐shot , 1998, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[52]  J. Doyon,et al.  Role of the striatum, cerebellum and frontal lobes in the automatization of a repeated visuomotor sequence of movements , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[53]  H. Damasio,et al.  Dissociation Of Working Memory from Decision Making within the Human Prefrontal Cortex , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[54]  D. Willingham A Neuropsychological Theory of Motor Skill Learning , 2004 .

[55]  S. Petersen,et al.  Changes in brain activity during motor learning measured with PET: effects of hand of performance and practice. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[56]  J. Desmond,et al.  The neural basis of visual skill learning: an fMRI study of mirror reading. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.

[57]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Transition of Brain Activation from Frontal to Parietal Areas in Visuomotor Sequence Learning , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[58]  T. Klingberg Concurrent performance of two working memory tasks: potential mechanisms of interference. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.

[59]  J. Desmond,et al.  Load-Dependent Roles of Frontal Brain Regions in the Maintenance of Working Memory , 1999, NeuroImage.

[60]  B. Knowlton What can neuropsychology tell us about category learning? , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.