Effects of Non-Symbolic Approximate Number Practice on Symbolic Numerical Abilities in Pakistani Children

Current theories of numerical cognition posit that uniquely human symbolic number abilities connect to an early developing cognitive system for representing approximate numerical magnitudes, the approximate number system (ANS). In support of this proposal, recent laboratory-based training experiments with U.S. children show enhanced performance on symbolic addition after brief practice comparing or adding arrays of dots without counting: tasks that engage the ANS. Here we explore the nature and generality of this effect through two brief training experiments. In Experiment 1, elementary school children in Pakistan practiced either a non-symbolic numerical addition task or a line-length addition task with no numerical content, and then were tested on symbolic addition. After training, children in the numerical training group completed the symbolic addition test faster than children in the line length training group, suggesting a causal role of brief, non-symbolic numerical training on exact, symbolic addition. These findings replicate and extend the core findings of a recent U.S. laboratory-based study to non-Western children tested in a school setting, attesting to the robustness and generalizability of the observed training effects. Experiment 2 tested whether ANS training would also enhance the consistency of performance on a symbolic number line task. Over several analyses of the data there was some evidence that approximate number training enhanced symbolic number line placements relative to control conditions. Together, the findings suggest that engagement of the ANS through brief training procedures enhances children's immediate attention to number and engagement with symbolic number tasks.

[1]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  Numerical ordering ability mediates the relation between number-sense and arithmetic competence , 2011, Cognition.

[2]  Ariel Starr,et al.  Number sense in infancy predicts mathematical abilities in childhood , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[3]  R. Siegler,et al.  The Development of Numerical Estimation , 2003, Psychological science.

[4]  Walter R. Boot,et al.  The Pervasive Problem With Placebos in Psychology , 2013, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[5]  S Dehaene,et al.  Attention, automaticity, and levels of representation in number processing. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[6]  L. Feigenson,et al.  Preschoolers' Precision of the Approximate Number System Predicts Later School Mathematics Performance , 2011, PloS one.

[7]  Julie L. Booth,et al.  Numerical magnitude representations influence arithmetic learning. , 2008, Child development.

[8]  Gavin R. Price,et al.  Impaired parietal magnitude processing in developmental dyscalculia , 2007, Current Biology.

[9]  Michael Schneider,et al.  Associations of non-symbolic and symbolic numerical magnitude processing with mathematical competence: a meta-analysis. , 2017, Developmental science.

[10]  H S Terrace,et al.  Ordering of the numerosities 1 to 9 by monkeys. , 1998, Science.

[11]  Stephan E. Vogel,et al.  Corrigendum to “Overlapping and distinct brain regions involved in estimating the spatial position of numerical and non-numerical magnitudes: An fMRI study” [Neuropsychologia 51 (2013) 979–989] , 2017, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Mapping numerical magnitudes onto symbols: the numerical distance effect and individual differences in children's mathematics achievement. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[13]  S. Gulati Technology-Enhanced Learning in Developing Nations: A Review. , 2008 .

[14]  T. Verguts,et al.  Dissecting the symbolic distance effect: Comparison and priming effects in numerical and nonnumerical orders , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[15]  Daniel C. Hyde,et al.  Small and large number discrimination in guppies , 2011, Animal Cognition.

[16]  Hubert Preissl,et al.  Magnetoencephalographic Signatures of Numerosity Discrimination in Fetuses and Neonates , 2014, Developmental neuropsychology.

[17]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Representation of number in the brain. , 2009, Annual review of neuroscience.

[18]  Manuela Piazza,et al.  Neurocognitive start-up tools for symbolic number representations , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[19]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  Elizabeth S Spelke,et al.  Children's expectations about training the approximate number system. , 2015, The British journal of developmental psychology.

[21]  Janosch Linkersdörfer,et al.  Symbolic and non-symbolic distance effects in children and their connection with arithmetic skills , 2011, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[22]  Kelly S. Mix,et al.  Thinking about quantity: the intertwined development of spatial and numerical cognition. , 2015, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[23]  Marie-Pascale Noël,et al.  Symbolic and nonsymbolic number comparison in children with and without dyscalculia , 2010, Cognition.

[24]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Impaired acuity of the approximate number system underlies mathematical learning disability (dyscalculia). , 2011, Child development.

[25]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Abstract representations of numbers in the animal and human brain , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.

[26]  Emmy Defever,et al.  Association between basic numerical abilities and mathematics achievement. , 2012, The British journal of developmental psychology.

[27]  Lisa K Fazio,et al.  Relations of different types of numerical magnitude representations to each other and to mathematics achievement. , 2014, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[28]  Susan Carey,et al.  One, two, three, four, nothing more: An investigation of the conceptual sources of the verbal counting principles , 2007, Cognition.

[29]  E. Spelke,et al.  Newborn infants perceive abstract numbers , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[30]  Julie L. Booth,et al.  Development of numerical estimation in young children. , 2004, Child development.

[31]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology : General Hysteresis Affects Approximate Number Discrimination in Young Children , 2012 .

[32]  E. Spelke,et al.  Language and Conceptual Development series Core systems of number , 2004 .

[33]  Arlette Streri,et al.  Dissociation between small and large numerosities in newborn infants. , 2014, Developmental science.

[34]  B. Reynvoet,et al.  Children's representation of symbolic and nonsymbolic magnitude examined with the priming paradigm. , 2011, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[35]  Robert S. Siegler,et al.  Overlapping and distinct brain regions involved in estimating the spatial position of numerical and non-numerical magnitudes: An fMRI study , 2013, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  Malleability of the approximate number system: effects of feedback and training , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[37]  Julie L. Booth,et al.  Developmental and individual differences in pure numerical estimation. , 2006, Developmental psychology.

[38]  Elizabeth M Brannon,et al.  Semantic congruity affects numerical judgments similarly in monkeys and humans. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[39]  E. Brannon The independence of language and mathematical reasoning. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[40]  Avishai Henik,et al.  Notation-Dependent and -Independent Representations of Numbers in the Parietal Lobes , 2007, Neuron.

[41]  Neil Marlow,et al.  Individual Differences in Inhibitory Control, Not Non-Verbal Number Acuity, Correlate with Mathematics Achievement , 2013, PloS one.

[42]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  The relation between spatial skill and early number knowledge: the role of the linear number line. , 2012, Developmental psychology.

[43]  Nicole M. McNeil,et al.  ANS acuity and mathematics ability in preschoolers from low-income homes: contributions of inhibitory control. , 2013, Developmental science.

[44]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Individual differences in non-verbal number acuity correlate with maths achievement , 2008, Nature.

[45]  Silke M. Göbel,et al.  Impact of High Mathematics Education on the Number Sense , 2012, PloS one.

[46]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  How do symbolic and non-symbolic numerical magnitude processing skills relate to individual differences in children's mathematical skills? A review of evidence from brain and behavior , 2013, Trends in Neuroscience and Education.

[47]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Beyond magnitude: Judging ordinality of symbolic number is unrelated to magnitude comparison and independently relates to individual differences in arithmetic , 2016, Cognition.

[48]  Melissa E. Libertus,et al.  Preschool acuity of the approximate number system correlates with school math ability. , 2011, Developmental science.

[49]  ROBERT S. MOYER,et al.  Time required for Judgements of Numerical Inequality , 1967, Nature.

[50]  Andrea Facoetti,et al.  Developmental trajectory of number acuity reveals a severe impairment in developmental dyscalculia , 2010, Cognition.

[51]  S. Carey The Origin of Concepts , 2000 .

[52]  Philippe Pinel,et al.  Distributed and Overlapping Cerebral Representations of Number, Size, and Luminance during Comparative Judgments , 2004, Neuron.

[53]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Cultural Recycling of Cortical Maps , 2007, Neuron.

[54]  Daniel C. Hyde,et al.  Brief non-symbolic, approximate number practice enhances subsequent exact symbolic arithmetic in children , 2014, Cognition.

[55]  M. Noël,et al.  Basic numerical skills in children with mathematics learning disabilities: A comparison of symbolic vs non-symbolic number magnitude processing , 2007, Cognition.

[56]  Halina T. Kobryn,et al.  Dynamic Stability of Coral Reefs on the West Australian Coast , 2013, PloS one.

[57]  H S Terrace,et al.  Representation of the numerosities 1-9 by rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[58]  Karen Wynn,et al.  Infants' Individuation and Enumeration of Actions , 1996 .

[59]  S. Dehaene,et al.  THREE PARIETAL CIRCUITS FOR NUMBER PROCESSING , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[60]  D. LeBihan,et al.  Modulation of Parietal Activation by Semantic Distance in a Number Comparison Task , 2001, NeuroImage.

[61]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Nonsymbolic numerical magnitude comparison: reliability and validity of different task variants and outcome measures, and their relationship to arithmetic achievement in adults. , 2012, Acta psychologica.

[62]  Yaoran Li,et al.  Acuity of the approximate number system and preschoolers' quantitative development. , 2014, Developmental science.

[63]  Elizabeth M Brannon,et al.  Training the Approximate Number System Improves Math Proficiency , 2013, Psychological science.

[64]  Qixuan Chen,et al.  Association between individual differences in non-symbolic number acuity and math performance: a meta-analysis. , 2014, Acta psychologica.

[65]  I. Berteletti,et al.  Approximate numerical abilities and mathematics: Insight from correlational and experimental training studies. , 2016, Progress in brain research.

[66]  Stella F. Lourenco,et al.  Nonsymbolic number and cumulative area representations contribute shared and unique variance to symbolic math competence , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[67]  S. Dehaene,et al.  A Magnitude Code Common to Numerosities and Number Symbols in Human Intraparietal Cortex , 2007, Neuron.

[68]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  Improving arithmetic performance with number sense training: An investigation of underlying mechanism , 2014, Cognition.