A Primate-Inspired Autonomous Navigation Algorithm Using the Cognitive Mechanism of Mental Rotation

Though significant progress on autonomous navigation has been made, the natural world offers interesting examples of navigational techniques that are worth exploring and understanding. The cognitive mechanism of mental rotation has been revealed in numerous cognitive and neuroscientific experiments; its reason for existence and evolution, however, has yet to be thoroughly understood. It is speculated that this mechanism may assist primates in navigation. This paper explores how mental rotation can be used in navigation by developing an autonomous robotic navigation algorithm that draws inspiration from the mechanism. This algorithm was tested on a robot tasked with navigating to a specified goal location contained within the agent's initial view. The testing suggests that mental rotation can be used as an asset in navigation.

[1]  Ronald C. Arkin,et al.  An Behavior-based Robotics , 1998 .

[2]  Stella F. Lourenco,et al.  Using geometry to specify location: implications for spatial coding in children and nonhuman animals , 2007, Psychological research.

[3]  Mary Hegarty,et al.  Representations of Shape during Mental Rotation , 2010, AAAI Spring Symposium: Cognitive Shape Processing.

[4]  Frank Dellaert,et al.  Primate-inspired mental rotations: Implications for robot control , 2012, 2012 IEEE International Conference on Robotics and Biomimetics (ROBIO).

[5]  Anthony J. Aretz,et al.  The Mental Rotation of Map Displays , 1992 .

[6]  C. Menzel,et al.  Memory and foraging theory: chimpanzee utilization of optimality heuristics in the rank-order recovery of hidden foods , 2012, Animal Behaviour.

[7]  Edward M. Riseman,et al.  Towards cosmopolitan robots: intelligent navigation in extended man-made environments , 1987 .

[8]  Lynn Andrea Stein,et al.  Imagination and situated cognition , 1991, J. Exp. Theor. Artif. Intell..

[9]  Emil W. Menzel,et al.  Do Primates Plan Routes? Simple Detour Problems Reconsidered. , 2007 .

[10]  C. Menzel,et al.  Unprompted recall and reporting of hidden objects by a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) after extended delays. , 1999, Journal of comparative psychology.

[11]  Wolfram Burgard,et al.  Probabilistic Robotics (Intelligent Robotics and Autonomous Agents) , 2005 .

[12]  E. Sue Savage-Rumbaugh,et al.  Bonobo (Pan paniscus) Spatial Memory and Communication in a 20-hectare Forest , 2002, International Journal of Primatology.

[13]  Frank Dellaert,et al.  A Rao-Blackwellized MCMC algorithm for recovering piecewise planar 3D models from multiple view RGBD images , 2014, 2014 IEEE International Conference on Image Processing (ICIP).

[14]  Frank Dellaert,et al.  Planar Segmentation of RGBD Images Using Fast Linear Fitting and Markov Chain Monte Carlo , 2012, 2012 Ninth Conference on Computer and Robot Vision.

[15]  C. Menzel,et al.  Structure‐guided foraging in long‐tailed macaques , 1996, American journal of primatology.

[16]  Ronald C. Arkin,et al.  Multiagent Mission Specification and Execution , 1997, Auton. Robots.

[17]  A. Hutcheson,et al.  From maps to navigation: The role of cues in finding locations in a virtual environment , 2012, Memory & cognition.

[18]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  The case for mental imagery , 2006 .

[19]  Peter Brugger,et al.  Mental rotation of primate hands: human-likeness and thumb saliency , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  Frank Dellaert,et al.  Primate-inspired vehicle navigation using optic flow and mental rotations , 2013, Defense, Security, and Sensing.

[21]  M. Hegarty,et al.  A dissociation between mental rotation and perspective-taking spatial abilities , 2004 .

[22]  Ronald C. Arkin,et al.  The role of mental rotations in primate-inspired robot navigation , 2012, Cognitive Processing.