The restricted influence of sparseness of coding on the capacity of familiarity discrimination networks

Much evidence indicates that the perirhinal cortex is involved in the familiarity discrimination aspect of recognition memory. It has been previously shown under selective conditions that neural networks performing familiarity discrimination can achieve very high storage capacity, being able to deal with many times more stimuli than associative memory networks can in associative recall. The capacity of associative memories for recall has been shown to be highly dependent on the sparseness of coding. However, previous work on the networks of Bogacz et al, Norman and O'Reilly and Sohal and Hasselmo that model familiarity discrimination in the perirhinal cortex has not investigated the effects of the sparseness of encoding on capacity. This paper explores how sparseness of coding influences the capacity of each of these published models and establishes that sparse coding influences the capacity of the different models in different ways. The capacity of the Bogacz et al model can be made independent of the sparseness of coding. Capacity increases as coding becomes sparser for a simplified version of the neocortical part of the Norman and O'Reilly model, whereas capacity decreases as coding becomes sparser for a simplified version of the Sohal and Hasselmo model. Thus in general, and in contrast to associative memory networks, sparse encoding results in little or no advantage for the capacity of familiarity discrimination networks. Hence it may be less important for coding to be sparse in the perirhinal cortex than it is in the hippocampus. Additionally, it is established that the capacities of the networks are strongly dependent on the precise form of the learning rules (synaptic plasticity) used in the network. This finding indicates that the precise characteristics of synaptic plastic changes in the real brain are likely to have major influences on storage capacity.

[1]  T. Bliss,et al.  A synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampus , 1993, Nature.

[2]  J. Buhmann,et al.  Associative memory with high information content. , 1989, Physical review. A, General physics.

[3]  W S McCulloch,et al.  A logical calculus of the ideas immanent in nervous activity , 1990, The Philosophy of Artificial Intelligence.

[4]  M. W. Brown,et al.  Episodic memory, amnesia, and the hippocampal–anterior thalamic axis , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[5]  Christophe Giraud-Carrier,et al.  High Capacity Neural Networks for Familiarity Discrimination , 1999 .

[6]  M. W. Brown,et al.  Recognition memory: neuronal substrates of the judgement of prior occurrence , 1998, Progress in Neurobiology.

[7]  R. Palmer,et al.  , Introduction to the Theory of Neural Computation 1 , 2007 .

[8]  L. Squire,et al.  The human perirhinal cortex and recognition memory , 1998, Hippocampus.

[9]  I. Riches,et al.  The effects of visual stimulation and memory on neurons of the hippocampal formation and the neighboring parahippocampal gyrus and inferior temporal cortex of the primate , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[10]  Peter Dayan,et al.  Optimal Plasticity from Matrix Memories: What Goes Up Must Come Down , 1990, Neural Computation.

[11]  R. Desimone,et al.  The representation of stimulus familiarity in anterior inferior temporal cortex. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Perception, memory, and emotion : frontiers in neuroscience , 1996 .

[13]  R. O’Reilly,et al.  Modeling hippocampal and neocortical contributions to recognition memory: a complementary-learning-systems approach. , 2003, Psychological review.

[14]  E. Rolls,et al.  Computational analysis of the role of the hippocampus in memory , 1994, Hippocampus.

[15]  W. Pitts,et al.  A Logical Calculus of the Ideas Immanent in Nervous Activity (1943) , 2021, Ideas That Created the Future.

[16]  T. Bussey,et al.  Perceptual–mnemonic functions of the perirhinal cortex , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[17]  Neal J. Cohen,et al.  Erratum: The hippocampal memory system and its functional components: Further explication and clarification (Behavioral and Brain Sciences (1994) 17:3 (500)) , 1996 .

[18]  R. Desimone,et al.  Activity of neurons in anterior inferior temporal cortex during a short- term memory task , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[19]  J. Ringo,et al.  Investigation of long term recognition and association memory in unit responses from inferotemporal cortex , 1993, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  R. Desimone,et al.  Clustering of perirhinal neurons with similar properties following visual experience in adult monkeys , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  Two functional components of the hippocampal memory system , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[22]  Y. Prigent [Long term depression]. , 1989, Annales medico-psychologiques.

[23]  P. Dayan,et al.  Optimising synaptic learning rules in linear associative memories , 1991, Biological Cybernetics.

[24]  D Marr,et al.  Simple memory: a theory for archicortex. , 1971, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[25]  David J. Willshaw,et al.  Improving recall from an associative memory , 1995, Biological Cybernetics.

[26]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  A model for experience-dependent changes in the responses of inferotemporal neurons , 2000, Network.

[27]  John J. Hopfield,et al.  Neural networks and physical systems with emergent collective computational abilities , 1999 .

[28]  David Willshaw,et al.  On setting unit thresholds in an incompletely connected associative net Network: Comput , 1993 .

[29]  Malcolm W. Brown,et al.  Recognition memory: What are the roles of the perirhinal cortex and hippocampus? , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[30]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Comparison of spatial and temporal characteristics of neuronal activity in sequential stages of hippocampal processing. , 1990, Progress in brain research.

[31]  H. C. LONGUET-HIGGINS,et al.  Non-Holographic Associative Memory , 1969, Nature.

[32]  Wulfram Gerstner,et al.  Spiking neurons , 1999 .

[33]  M. W. Brown,et al.  Neuronal activity related to visual recognition memory: long-term memory and the encoding of recency and familiarity information in the primate anterior and medial inferior temporal and rhinal cortex , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[34]  Rafal Bogacz,et al.  Comparison of computational models of familiarity discrimination in the perirhinal cortex , 2003, Hippocampus.

[35]  Anders Krogh,et al.  Introduction to the theory of neural computation , 1994, The advanced book program.

[36]  M. W. Brown,et al.  Neuronal evidence that inferomedial temporal cortex is more important than hippocampus in certain processes underlying recognition memory , 1987, Brain Research.

[37]  J. Aggleton,et al.  Amnesia and recognition memory: A re-analysis of psychometric data , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[38]  Günther Palm,et al.  Information capacity in recurrent McCulloch-Pitts networks with sparsely coded memory states , 1992 .

[39]  M. W. Brown,et al.  Differential neuronal encoding of novelty, familiarity and recency in regions of the anterior temporal lobe , 1998, Neuropharmacology.

[40]  Erkki Oja,et al.  Adaptation of a linear system to a finite set of patterns occurring in an arbitrarily varying order , 1974 .

[41]  Z. Bashir,et al.  Long-term depression: a cascade of induction and expression mechanisms , 2001, Progress in Neurobiology.

[42]  Elisabeth A. Murray,et al.  What have ablation studies told us about the neural substrates of stimulus memory , 1996 .

[43]  C. Koch,et al.  Category-specific visual responses of single neurons in the human medial temporal lobe , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[44]  Rafal Bogacz,et al.  Model of Familiarity Discrimination in the Perirhinal Cortex , 2004, Journal of Computational Neuroscience.