Dissociating entorhinal and hippocampal involvement in latent inhibition.

This study used anatomical cues to suggest a functional dissociation between the roles of the entorhinal cortex and the hippocampus in learning. The authors proposed that the highly convergent inputs to the entorhinal cortex indicate this region may be particularly important for selecting or compressing information. This hypothesis was tested in rabbits (Oryctolagus cunniculus) trained on an associative learning task that is a common index of stimulus selection. In this task, known as latent inhibition, preexposure to a stimulus (such as a tone) leads to slowed learning when the same tone is subsequently paired with an outcome (such as an airpuff to the eye). As hypothesized, rabbits with neurotoxic lesions of the entorhinal cortex failed to show slowed learning following preexposure (no latent inhibition) and learned the association faster than control rabbits. In contrast, hippocampal-lesioned animals showed normal (slowed) learning.

[1]  E. Coutureau,et al.  Entorhinal but Not Hippocampal or Subicular Lesions Disrupt Latent Inhibition in Rats , 1999, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[2]  C. Buhusi,et al.  Perplexing effects of hippocampal lesions on latent inhibition: a neural network solution. , 1998, Behavioral neuroscience.

[3]  R. Lubow Latent inhibition as a measure of learned inattention: some problems and solutions , 1997, Behavioural Brain Research.

[4]  M. Gabriel,et al.  Lesions of the entorhinal cortex disrupt behavioral and neuronal responses to context change during extinction of discriminative avoidance behavior , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[5]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  Memory Representation within the Parahippocampal Region , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  R. C. Honey,et al.  Dissociable effects of selective lesions to hippocampal subsystems on exploratory behavior, contextual learning, and spatial learning. , 1997, Behavioral neuroscience.

[7]  Michael S. Fanselow,et al.  Electrolytic Lesions of the Fimbria/Fornix, Dorsal Hippocampus, or Entorhinal Cortex Produce Anterograde Deficits in Contextual Fear Conditioning in Rats , 1997, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[8]  M. Gallagher,et al.  Hippocampal lesions disrupt decrements but not increments in conditioned stimulus processing , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[9]  M. W. Brown,et al.  Changes in neuronal activity related to the repetition and relative familiarity of visual stimuli in rhinal and adjacent cortex of the anaesthetised rat , 1995, Brain Research.

[10]  R G Phillips,et al.  Lesions of the fornix but not the entorhinal or perirhinal cortex interfere with contextual fear conditioning , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[11]  M. Gluck,et al.  Dissociation of hippocampal and entorhinal function in associative learning: A computational approach , 1995, Psychobiology.

[12]  D. Amaral,et al.  Perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices of the macaque monkey: Cortical afferents , 1994, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[13]  John F. Disterhoft,et al.  A system for quantitative analysis of associative learning. Part 1. Hardware interfaces with cross-species applications , 1994, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[14]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  Two functional components of the hippocampal memory system , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[15]  W. Suzuki,et al.  Topographic organization of the reciprocal connections between the monkey entorhinal cortex and the perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[16]  N. Schmajuk,et al.  Latent inhibition of the rat eyeblink response: Effect of hippocampal aspiration lesions , 1994, Physiology & Behavior.

[17]  C. Harley,et al.  Ibotenate lesions of the hippocampus enhance latent inhibition in conditioned taste aversion and increase resistance to extinction in conditioned taste preference. , 1993, Behavioral neuroscience.

[18]  M. Gluck,et al.  Hippocampal mediation of stimulus representation: A computational theory , 1993, Hippocampus.

[19]  H Eichenbaum,et al.  Critical role of the parahippocampal region for paired-associate learning in rats. , 1993, Behavioral neuroscience.

[20]  L. Jarrard On the role of the hippocampus in learning and memory in the rat. , 1993, Behavioral and neural biology.

[21]  D. Amaral,et al.  Lesions of the perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices in the monkey produce long-lasting memory impairment in the visual and tactual modalities , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[22]  R. C. Honey,et al.  Selective hippocampal lesions abolish the contextual specificity of latent inhibition and conditioning. , 1993, Behavioral neuroscience.

[23]  L. Squire,et al.  Enduring memory impairment in monkeys after ischemic damage to the hippocampus , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[24]  H Eichenbaum,et al.  The theory that wouldn't die: A critical look at the spatial mapping theory of hippocampal function , 1991, Hippocampus.

[25]  B T Hyman,et al.  Some cytoarchitectural abnormalities of the entorhinal cortex in schizophrenia. , 1991, Archives of general psychiatry.

[26]  A. R. Damasio,et al.  Memory‐related neural systems in Alzheimer's disease , 1990, Neurology.

[27]  M. Witter,et al.  Functional organization of the extrinsic and intrinsic circuitry of the parahippocampal region , 1989, Progress in Neurobiology.

[28]  R. Sutherland,et al.  The hippocampal formation is necessary for rats to learn and remember configural discriminations , 1989, Behavioural Brain Research.

[29]  D. Amaral,et al.  The entorhinal cortex of the monkey: II. Cortical afferents , 1987, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[30]  J. Pearce,et al.  Hippocampal Lesions Attenuate Latent Inhibition and the Decline of the Orienting Response in Rats , 1987, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[31]  G. V. Hoesen,et al.  The parahippocampal gyrus: New observations regarding its cortical connections in the monkey , 1982, Trends in Neurosciences.

[32]  R. Morris,et al.  Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions , 1982, Nature.

[33]  M. Eckardt The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map , 1980 .

[34]  A. G. Baker Learned Irrelevance and Learned Helplessness: Rats Learn that Stimuli, Reinforcers, and Responses are Uncorrelated. , 1976 .

[35]  R. Hirsh The hippocampus and contextual retrieval of information from memory: a theory. , 1974, Behavioral biology.

[36]  G. Frommer,et al.  Effects of CS preexposures on avoidance learning in rats with hippocampal lesions. , 1969, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[37]  P. Schnur,et al.  Latent inhibition in human eyelid conditioning. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.

[38]  L. T. Robertson Memory and the brain. , 2002, Journal of dental education.

[39]  R. Sutherland,et al.  Configural association theory and the hippocampal formation: An appraisal and reconfiguration , 1995, Hippocampus.

[40]  J. Rawlins,et al.  Latent inhibition in rats is abolished by NMDA-induced neuronal loss in the retrohippocampal region, but this lesion effect can be prevented by systemic haloperidol treatment. , 1995, Behavioral neuroscience.

[41]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  Complementary roles of the orbital prefrontal cortex and the perirhinal-entorhinal cortices in an odor-guided delayed-nonmatching-to-sample task. , 1992, Behavioral neuroscience.

[42]  R. Lubow Latent inhibition. , 1973, Psychological bulletin.