Dynamic adaptation to history of trial difficulty explains the effect of congruency proportion on masked priming.

In reaction time research, there has been an increasing appreciation that response-initiation processes are sensitive to recent experience and, in particular, the difficulty of previous trials. From this perspective, the authors propose an explanation for a perplexing property of masked priming: Although primes are not consciously identified, facilitation of target processing by a related prime is magnified in a block containing a high proportion of related primes and a low proportion of unrelated primes relative to a block containing the opposite mix (Bodner & Masson, 2001). In the present study, this phenomenon is explored with a parity (even/odd) decision task in which a prime (e.g., 2) precedes a target that can be either congruent (e.g., 4) or incongruent (e.g., 3). It is shown that the effect of congruence proportion with masked primes cannot be explained in terms of the blockwise prime-target contingency. Specifically, with masked primes, there is no congruency disadvantage in a block containing a high proportion of incongruent primes, but there is a congruency advantage when the block contains an equal proportion of congruent and incongruent primes. In qualitative contrast, visible primes are sensitive to the blockwise prime-target contingency. The authors explain the relatedness proportion effect found with masked primes in terms of a model according to which response-initiation processes adapt to the statistical structure of the environment, specifically the difficulty of recent trials. This account is supported with an analysis at the level of individual trials using the linear mixed effects model.

[1]  R M Shiffrin,et al.  Perception and preference in short-term word priming. , 2001, Psychological review.

[2]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The physics of optimal decision making: a formal analysis of models of performance in two-alternative forced-choice tasks. , 2006, Psychological review.

[3]  D. Geary,et al.  Psychonomic Bulletin Review , 2000 .

[4]  R. Shillcock,et al.  Proceedings of the Twenty-Sixth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society , 1998 .

[5]  J. H. Neely Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. , 1991 .

[6]  M. Masson,et al.  Prime Validity Affects Masked Repetition Priming: Evidence for an Episodic Resource Account of Priming , 2001 .

[7]  K. Forster,et al.  REPETITION PRIMING AND FREQUENCY ATTENUATION IN LEXICAL ACCESS , 1984 .

[8]  R. Remington Analysis of sequential effects in choice reaction times. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.

[9]  AÂ rni KristjaÂnsson Rapid learning in attention shifts : A review , 2005 .

[10]  Dennis Norris,et al.  Perception as evidence accumulation and Bayesian inference: insights from masked priming. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[11]  R. Baayen,et al.  How cross-language similarity and task demands affect cognate recognition , 2010 .

[12]  Michael C. Mozer,et al.  Control of Response Initiation: Mechanisms of Adaptation to Recent Experience , 2004 .

[13]  Dennis Norris,et al.  Masked priming effect reflects evidence accumulated by the prime , 2010, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[14]  Elisabeth Dévière,et al.  Analyzing linguistic data: a practical introduction to statistics using R , 2009 .

[15]  S. Klapp Nonconscious control mimics a purposeful strategy: strength of Stroop-like interference is automatically modulated by proportion of compatible trials. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  Max Coltheart,et al.  Cross-task strategic effects , 2003, Memory & cognition.

[17]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Basic processes in reading : visual word recognition , 1993 .

[18]  Kenneth I Forster,et al.  DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy , 2003, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[19]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Human memory: An adaptive perspective. , 1989 .

[20]  Manuel Perea,et al.  Does the proportion of associatively related pairs modulate the associative priming effect at very brief stimulus-onset asynchronies? , 2002, Acta psychologica.

[21]  Glen E Bodner,et al.  Repetition proportion biases masked priming of lexical decisions , 2006, Memory & cognition.

[22]  O. Bagasra,et al.  Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences , 1914, Science.

[23]  Matt Jones,et al.  Sequential Effects Reflect Parallel Learning of Multiple Environmental Regularities , 2022 .

[24]  Michael C. Mozer,et al.  Sequential Dependencies in Human Behavior Offer Insights Into Cognitive Control , 2007, Integrated Models of Cognitive Systems.

[25]  E. Donchin,et al.  Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[26]  Glen E Bodner,et al.  Masked repetition priming and proportion effects under cognitive load. , 2008, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[27]  D Laming,et al.  Autocorrelation of choice-reaction times. , 1979, Acta psychologica.

[28]  K. Forster,et al.  Unconscious cognition isn’t that smart: Modulation of masked repetition priming effect in the word naming task , 2008, Cognition.

[29]  David A. Balota,et al.  Beyond mean response latency: Response time distributional analyses of semantic priming , 2008 .

[30]  Zenzi M. Griffin,et al.  The persistence of structural priming: transient activation or implicit learning? , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[31]  HighWire Press Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London , 1781, The London Medical Journal.

[32]  J. H. Neely Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. , 1977 .

[33]  Cynthia S. Sahm,et al.  Past trials influence perception of ambiguous motion quartets through pattern completion. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[34]  N. Anderson Effect of first-order conditional probability in two-choice learning situation. , 1960, Journal of experimental psychology.

[35]  J. Raaijmakers,et al.  Associative priming in a masked perceptual identification task: Evidence for automatic processes , 2002, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[36]  Drew H. Abney,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology : Human Perception and Performance Influence of Musical Groove on Postural Sway , 2015 .

[37]  M. Mozer,et al.  How lexical decision is affected by recent experience: Symmetric versus asymmetric frequency-blocking effects , 2006, Memory & cognition.

[38]  Antje S. Meyer,et al.  Word length effects in object naming: The role of a response criterion , 2003 .

[39]  Derek Besner,et al.  Contingency learning without awareness: Evidence for implicit control , 2007, Consciousness and Cognition.

[40]  R. Verleger,et al.  How the Self Controls Its Automatic Pilot when Processing Subliminal Information , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[41]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  The Diffusion Decision Model: Theory and Data for Two-Choice Decision Tasks , 2008, Neural Computation.

[42]  Frank E. Ritter,et al.  The Rise of Cognitive Architectures , 2007, Integrated Models of Cognitive Systems.

[43]  Matt Jones,et al.  Optimal Response Initiation: Why Recent Experience Matters , 2008, NIPS.

[44]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems 30 , 1995 .

[45]  Douglas Vickers,et al.  Dynamic Models of Simple Judgments: I. Properties of a Self-Regulating Accumulator Module , 1998 .

[46]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[47]  P. Lemaire,et al.  Age-related differences in automatic stimulus-response associations: Insights from young and older adults’ parity judgments , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[48]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Event perception and the word repetition effect , 1988 .

[49]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  A computational model of anterior cingulate function in speeded response tasks: Effects of frequency, sequence, and conflict , 2002, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[50]  Glen E Bodner,et al.  Masked priming of number judgments depends on prime validity and task , 2005, Memory & cognition.

[51]  D. M. Green,et al.  Sequential effects in judgments of loudness. , 1977, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[52]  Peter Dixon,et al.  Perseveration and contrast effects in grasping , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[53]  B. W. Whittlesea,et al.  Implicit /explicit memory versus analytic/nonanalytic processing: Rethinking the mere exposure effect , 2001, Memory & cognition.

[54]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  Automatic stimulus-response associations may be semantically mediated , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[55]  M. Masson,et al.  Beyond binary judgments: Prime validity modulates masked repetition priming in the naming task , 2004, Memory & cognition.

[56]  M. Masson,et al.  Beyond spreading activation: An influence of relatedness proportion on masked semantic priming , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[57]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .

[58]  K I Forster,et al.  The Pros and Cons of Masked Priming , 1998, Journal of psycholinguistic research.

[59]  S. Lupker,et al.  Sequential effects in naming: a time-criterion account. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[60]  R. Baayen,et al.  Reading polymorphemic Dutch compounds: toward a multiple route model of lexical processing. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[61]  R. Kronauer,et al.  Affective Discrimination of Stimuli That Cannot Be Recognized , 2022 .

[62]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Reflections of the Environment in Memory Form of the Memory Functions , 2022 .

[63]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Sequential effects: Superstition or rational behavior? , 2008, NIPS.

[64]  Michael E. J. Masson,et al.  A Retrospective View of Masked Priming : Toward a Unified Account of Masked and Long-Term Repetition Priming , 2003 .

[65]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Levels of processing during non-conscious perception: a critical review of visual masking , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[66]  A. Glass,et al.  Context Effects in Sentence Verification. , 1981 .

[67]  S. Lupker,et al.  Strategic Control in a Naming Task: Changing Routes or Changing Deadlines? , 1997 .

[68]  Manuel Perea,et al.  Does letter position coding depend on consonant/vowel status? Evidence with the masked priming technique. , 2009, Acta psychologica.

[69]  Giordana Grossi Relatedness proportion effects on masked associative priming: an ERP study. , 2006, Psychophysiology.

[70]  Winston R. Sieck,et al.  Learning myopia: an adaptive recency effect in category learning. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[71]  M. Coltheart,et al.  Mixing costs and mixing benefits in naming words, pictures and sums , 2003 .

[72]  W. Estes Toward a Statistical Theory of Learning. , 1994 .

[73]  M Brysbaert,et al.  Prelexical phonological coding of visual words in Dutch: Automatic after all , 2001, Memory & cognition.