Computational model of excitatory/inhibitory ratio imbalance role in attention deficit disorders

Impairments in attentional behaviors, including over-selectivity, under-selectivity, distractibility and difficulty in shift of attention, are widely reported in several developmental disorders, including autism. Uncharacteristic inhibitory to excitatory neuronal number ratio (IER) and abnormal synaptic strength levels in the brain are two broadly accepted neurobiological disorders observed in autistic individuals. These neurobiological findings are contrasting and their relation to the atypical attentional behaviors is not clear yet. In this paper, we take a computational approach to investigate the relation of imbalanced IER and abnormal synaptic strength to some well-documented spectrum of attentional impairments. The computational model is based on a modified version of a biologically plausible neural model of two competing minicolumns in IT cortex augmented with a simple model of top-down attention. Top-down attention is assumed to amplify (attenuates) attended (unattended) stimulus. The inhibitory synaptic strength parameter in the model is set such that typical attentional behavior is emerged. Then, according to related findings, the parameter is changed and the model’s attentional behavior is considered. The simulation results show that, without any change in top-down attention, the abnormal inhibitory synaptic strength values – and IER imbalance- result in over-selectivity, under-selectivity, distractibility and difficulty in shift of attention in the model. It suggests that the modeled neurobiological abnormalities can be accounted for the attentional deficits. In addition, the atypical attentional behaviors do not necessarily point to impairments in top-down attention. Our simulations suggest that limited changes in the inhibitory synaptic strength and variations in top-down attention signal affect the model’s attentional behaviors in the same way. So, limited deficits in the inhibitory strength may be alleviated by appropriate change in top-down attention biasing. Nevertheless, our model proposes that this compensation is not possible for very high and very low values of the inhibitory strength.

[1]  E Courchesne,et al.  Attention function and dysfunction in autism. , 2001, Frontiers in bioscience : a journal and virtual library.

[2]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Attention Speeds Binocular Rivalry , 2006, Psychological science.

[3]  Rosalyn Shute,et al.  An analysis of stimulus overselectivity in adults with autism , 2001 .

[4]  David L. Sheinberg,et al.  Visual object recognition. , 1996, Annual review of neuroscience.

[5]  M. Belmonte,et al.  Abnormal Attention in Autism Shown by Steady-State Visual Evoked Potentials , 2000 .

[6]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  The neural basis of biased competition in human visual cortex , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  Robert Coben,et al.  The Relative Efficacy of Connectivity Guided and Symptom Based EEG Biofeedback for Autistic Disorders , 2010, Applied psychophysiology and biofeedback.

[8]  W. Levelt On binocular rivalry , 1965 .

[9]  J L Bradshaw,et al.  A Deficit in Shifting Attention Present in High-Functioning Autism but not Asperger’s Disorder , 2001, Autism : the international journal of research and practice.

[10]  O. I. Lovaas,et al.  Stimulus overselectivity: a common feature in autism and mental retardation. , 1976, American journal of mental deficiency.

[11]  E. Rolls,et al.  Neurodynamics of biased competition and cooperation for attention: a model with spiking neurons. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  Paul H. E. Tiesinga,et al.  Attentional modulation of firing rate and synchrony in a model cortical network , 2005, Journal of Computational Neuroscience.

[13]  A. Carlsson,et al.  The 5-HT2A receptor antagonist M100907 is more effective in counteracting NMDA antagonist- than dopamine agonist-induced hyperactivity in mice , 1999, Journal of Neural Transmission.

[14]  J. R. Hughes Update on autism: A review of 1300 reports published in 2008 , 2009, Epilepsy & Behavior.

[15]  Marco Congedo,et al.  Neurofeedback Improves Executive Functioning in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. , 2009 .

[16]  Eric Courchesne,et al.  Parietal Damage and Narrow Spotlight Spatial Attention , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[17]  Cynthia R. Johnson,et al.  Attentional Processes in Autism , 2001, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[18]  R. Desimone,et al.  Attention Increases Sensitivity of V4 Neurons , 2000, Neuron.

[19]  Geoffrey Bird,et al.  Attention does not modulate neural responses to social stimuli in autism spectrum disorders , 2006, NeuroImage.

[20]  R. Desimone,et al.  The Effects of Visual Stimulation and Selective Visual Attention on Rhythmic Neuronal Synchronization in Macaque Area V4 , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  M. Merzenich,et al.  Model of autism: increased ratio of excitation/inhibition in key neural systems , 2003, Genes, brain, and behavior.

[22]  C. Olson,et al.  Low-frequency oscillations arising from competitive interactions between visual stimuli in macaque inferotemporal cortex. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  Thomas E. Myers,et al.  Neurofeedback for Autistic Spectrum Disorder: A Review of the Literature , 2010, Applied psychophysiology and biofeedback.

[24]  L. Schreibman,et al.  Selective responding by autistic children to multiple sensory input. , 1971, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[25]  Sarah White,et al.  Big heads, small details and autism , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[26]  Xiao-Jing Wang,et al.  Probabilistic Decision Making by Slow Reverberation in Cortical Circuits , 2002, Neuron.

[27]  Timothy J Andrews,et al.  The Role of Voluntary and Involuntary Attention in Selecting Perceptual Dominance during Binocular Rivalry , 2007, Perception.

[28]  Matthew K Belmonte,et al.  Functional anatomy of impaired selective attention and compensatory processing in autism. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[29]  R. Desimone,et al.  Responses of Neurons in Inferior Temporal Cortex during Memory- Guided Visual Search , 1998 .

[30]  Betty Jarusiewicz,et al.  Efficacy of Neurofeedback for Children in the Autistic Spectrum: A Pilot Study , 2002 .

[31]  H. Engeland,et al.  Minicolumnar abnormalities in autism , 2006, Acta Neuropathologica.

[32]  R. Blake,et al.  Endogenous attention prolongs dominance durations in binocular rivalry. , 2005, Journal of vision.

[33]  Phil Reed,et al.  The Effect of Concurrent Task Load on Stimulus Over-Selectivity , 2005, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[34]  J. R. Hughes,et al.  A review of recent reports on autism: 1000 studies published in 2007 , 2008, Epilepsy & Behavior.

[35]  Carson C. Chow,et al.  Competitive dynamics in cortical responses to visual stimuli. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[37]  R. Desimone Visual attention mediated by biased competition in extrastriate visual cortex. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[38]  K. Plaisted,et al.  Top-Down Attentional Modulation in Autistic Spectrum Disorders Is Stimulus-Specific , 2005, Psychological science.

[39]  Manuel Casanova,et al.  Radial cytoarchitecture and patterns of cortical connectivity in autism , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[40]  B. J. Casey,et al.  Dysfunctional attention in autistic savants. , 1993, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[41]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Visual competition , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[42]  Chris L. E. Paffen,et al.  Attentional Modulation of Binocular Rivalry , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[43]  Andrew E. Switala,et al.  Minicolumnar pathology in autism , 2002, Neurology.

[44]  Gustavo Deco,et al.  The Neuronal Basis of Attention: Rate versus Synchronization Modulation , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  G. Baird,et al.  The frequency and distribution of spontaneous attention shifts between social and nonsocial stimuli in autistic, typically developing, and nonautistic developmentally delayed infants. , 1998, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[46]  J. Démos Getting Started with Neurofeedback , 2005 .

[47]  J. Pevsner,et al.  Postmortem brain abnormalities of the glutamate neurotransmitter system in autism , 2001, Neurology.

[48]  G. DeAngelis,et al.  A Normalization Model of Multisensory Integration , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[49]  S. Bryson,et al.  Impaired disengagement of attention in young children with autism. , 2004, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[50]  M. Eric Gershwin,et al.  Genetic and Immunologic Considerations in Autism , 2002, Neurobiology of Disease.

[51]  P. Tiesinga,et al.  Role of interneuron diversity in the cortical microcircuit for attention. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[52]  Geraldine Dawson,et al.  Attention and the socioemotional impairments of individuals with autism , 1989 .

[53]  E. Niebur,et al.  Growth patterns in the developing brain detected by using continuum mechanical tensor maps , 2022 .

[54]  S. Epstein,et al.  Gamma oscillations mediate stimulus competition and attentional selection in a cortical network model , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[55]  Carl R. Olson,et al.  Inferotemporal Cortex Interactions Between Visual Stimuli in Macaque Low-Frequency Oscillations Arising From Competitive , 2005 .

[56]  Mike Coleman,et al.  Selective Attention and Perceptual Load in Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2009, Psychological science.

[57]  D. Buxhoeveden,et al.  The minicolumn hypothesis in neuroscience. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[58]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Object category structure in response patterns of neuronal population in monkey inferior temporal cortex. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[59]  E K Perry,et al.  Autism as a disorder of neural information processing: directions for research and targets for therapy* , 2004, Molecular Psychiatry.

[60]  R. Desimone,et al.  Modulation of Oscillatory Neuronal Synchronization by Selective Visual Attention , 2001, Science.

[61]  Ruggieri Vl Atentional processes and attention deficit disorders in autism , 2006 .

[62]  T. Bourgeron,et al.  Linkage and association of the glutamate receptor 6 gene with autism , 2002, Molecular Psychiatry.

[63]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Inferotemporal cortex and object vision. , 1996, Annual review of neuroscience.

[64]  Ignace T C Hooge,et al.  The effect of set size on the dynamics of binocular rivalry. , 2011, Seeing and perceiving.

[65]  S. Baron-Cohen Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind , 1997 .

[66]  E Courchesne,et al.  Effects of focused selective attention tasks on event-related potentials in autistic and normal individuals. , 1990, Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology.

[67]  F. Polleux,et al.  Toward a developmental neurobiology of autism. , 2004, Mental retardation and developmental disabilities research reviews.

[68]  Victor Ruggieri,et al.  Procesos atencionales y trastornos por déficit de atención en el autismo , 2006 .

[69]  Koorosh Mirpour,et al.  State-dependent effects of stimulus presentation duration on the temporal dynamics of neural responses in the inferotemporal cortex of macaque monkeys. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[70]  D. Broadbent Perception and communication , 1958 .

[71]  R. Desimone,et al.  Competitive Mechanisms Subserve Attention in Macaque Areas V2 and V4 , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.