A Model of Visual Motion Processing in Area MT of Primates

Motion perception requires the visual system to satisfy two conflicting demands: first, spatial integration of signals from neighboring regions of the visual field to overcome noisy signals, and second, sensitivity to small velocity differences to segment regions corresponding to different objects (Braddick, 1993). We have developed a computational model for the visual processing of motion in area MT that accounts for these conflicting demands. The model has two types of units, similar to those found in area MT. One type of unit in the model integrates information about the direction motion to estimate the local velocity; these local velocity units compete among themselves to determine the most likely local velocity. A second type of unit selects regions of the visual field where the velocity estimates are most reliable; these selection units have nonclassical receptive field surrounds by virtue of competition with pools of similar units across the visual field. The output of the model is a distributed segmentation of the image into patches that support distinct objects moving with a common velocity. T h e processing of motion in the primate's visual cortex begins in area V1, where cells with reliable selec-tivity for direction of motion are found (Maunsell and Newsome, 1987); however, these cells d o not detect true velocity but instead are tuned to a limited range of spatiotemporal frequencies and exhibit spatially restricted receptive fields so that they can report only the perpendicular component of the velocity for straight edges. This so-called aperture problem is illustrated in figure 27.1. T o overcome these limitations and compute true local velocity measurements, it is necessary to integrate motion responses from cells with a variety of directions and spatiotemporal frequency tunings over a Neurons that respond selectively to velocity over a wide range of spatial frequencies are found in visual area M T , which receives a direct projection from area A class of cells in M T , the "pattern cells" of Movshon and colleagues (1985), respond to the direction of overall motion of plaid patterns composed of two differently oriented gratings rather than to the direction of the individual components. Psychophysical studies suggest that the perceived velocity of such patterns generally is close to the velocity that is uniquely consistent with the constraints imposed by the individual component's motions (Adelson and Movshon, 1982), although other possibilities have been suggested (Wilson et al., 1992; Rubin and Hochstein, 1993). In …

[1]  Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al.  Filter Selection Model for Generating Visual Motion Signals , 1992, NIPS.

[2]  D C Van Essen,et al.  Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. I. Selectivity for stimulus direction, speed, and orientation. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  M. Morgan,et al.  Conditions for motion flow in dynamic visual noise , 1980, Vision Research.

[4]  Kevan A. C. Martin,et al.  A Canonical Microcircuit for Neocortex , 1989, Neural Computation.

[5]  L. Palmer,et al.  Contribution of linear spatiotemporal receptive field structure to velocity selectivity of simple cells in area 17 of cat , 1989, Vision Research.

[6]  BELA JULESZ,et al.  Rapid discrimination of visual patterns , 1983, IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics.

[7]  A. Yuille,et al.  A model for the estimate of local image velocity by cells in the visual cortex , 1990, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. B. Biological Sciences.

[8]  Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al.  Filter selection model for motion segmentation and velocity integration , 1994 .

[9]  E. Adelson,et al.  The analysis of moving visual patterns , 1985 .

[10]  Norberto M. Grzywacz,et al.  A Local Model for Transparent Motions Based on Spatio-Temporal Filters , 1993 .

[11]  R. Shapley,et al.  Quantitative analysis of retinal ganglion cell classifications. , 1976, The Journal of physiology.

[12]  E. Adelson,et al.  Directionally selective complex cells and the computation of motion energy in cat visual cortex , 1992, Vision Research.

[13]  D Marr,et al.  Directional selectivity and its use in early visual processing , 1981, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[14]  R A Andersen,et al.  The response of area MT and V1 neurons to transparent motion , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[15]  T. D. Albright,et al.  Transparency and coherence in human motion perception , 1990, Nature.

[16]  E H Adelson,et al.  Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[17]  Leslie Welch,et al.  The perception of moving plaids reveals two motion-processing stages , 1989, Nature.

[18]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Integration of direction signals of image motion in the superior temporal sulcus of the macaque monkey , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[19]  A. Treisman Features and Objects: The Fourteenth Bartlett Memorial Lecture , 1988, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[20]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Psychophysical isolation of movement sensitivity by removal of familiar position cues , 1981, Vision Research.

[21]  T. Albright Direction and orientation selectivity of neurons in visual area MT of the macaque. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  J. Maunsell,et al.  Visual processing in monkey extrastriate cortex. , 1987, Annual review of neuroscience.

[23]  C Koch,et al.  Computing motion in the primate's visual system. , 1989, The Journal of experimental biology.

[24]  田中 啓治 Analysis of Local and Wide-Field Movements in the Superior Temporal Visual Areas of the Macaque Monkey , 1987 .

[25]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  Motion capture anisotropy , 1987, Vision Research.

[26]  Klein,et al.  Nonlinear directionally selective subunits in complex cells of cat striate cortex. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  D. G. Albrecht,et al.  Motion selectivity and the contrast-response function of simple cells in the visual cortex , 1991, Visual Neuroscience.

[28]  L. Maffei,et al.  Spatial frequency rows in the striate visual cortex , 1977, Vision Research.

[29]  O. Braddick A short-range process in apparent motion. , 1974, Vision research.

[30]  O. Braddick Segmentation versus integration in visual motion processing , 1993, Trends in Neurosciences.

[31]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  Thalamocortical oscillations in the sleeping and aroused brain. , 1993, Science.

[32]  D J Heeger,et al.  Model for the extraction of image flow. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[33]  Hans-Hellmut Nagel,et al.  On the Estimation of Optical Flow: Relations between Different Approaches and Some New Results , 1987, Artif. Intell..

[34]  H. Wilson,et al.  A psychophysically motivated model for two-dimensional motion perception , 1992, Visual Neuroscience.

[35]  R. Born,et al.  Segregation of global and local motion processing in primate middle temporal visual area , 1993, Nature.

[36]  E. Adelson,et al.  Phenomenal coherence of moving visual patterns , 1982, Nature.

[37]  Jerome A. Feldman,et al.  Connectionist Models and Their Properties , 1982, Cogn. Sci..

[38]  Dana H. Ballard,et al.  Active Perception and Reinforcement Learning , 1990, Neural Computation.

[39]  T D Albright,et al.  Form-cue invariant motion processing in primate visual cortex. , 1992, Science.

[40]  R. Holub,et al.  Response of Visual Cortical Neurons of the cat to moving sinusoidal gratings: response-contrast functions and spatiotemporal interactions. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  D. Heeger Normalization of cell responses in cat striate cortex , 1992, Visual Neuroscience.

[42]  Shaul Hochstein,et al.  Isolating the effect of one-dimensional motion signals on the perceived direction of moving two-dimensional objects , 1993, Vision Research.

[43]  K. H. Britten,et al.  Neuronal correlates of a perceptual decision , 1989, Nature.

[44]  J. Allman,et al.  Stimulus specific responses from beyond the classical receptive field: neurophysiological mechanisms for local-global comparisons in visual neurons. , 1985, Annual review of neuroscience.

[45]  W. Newsome,et al.  Motion selectivity in macaque visual cortex. II. Spatiotemporal range of directional interactions in MT and V1. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  D. Pollen,et al.  Relationship between spatial frequency selectivity and receptive field profile of simple cells. , 1979, The Journal of physiology.

[47]  I. Ohzawa,et al.  Contrast gain control in the cat's visual system. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  C. Gross,et al.  Visual topography of striate projection zone (MT) in posterior superior temporal sulcus of the macaque. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[49]  Robert Sekuler,et al.  Coherent global motion percepts from stochastic local motions , 1984, Vision Research.

[50]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  A critique of pure vision , 1993 .

[51]  R. Desimone,et al.  Selective attention gates visual processing in the extrastriate cortex. , 1985, Science.