Egocentric Spaw Representation in Early Vision

Recent physiological experiments have shown that the responses of many neurons in V1 and V3a are modulated by the direction of gaze. We have developed a neural network model of the hierarchy of maps in visual cortex to explore the hypothesis that visual features are encoded in egocentric (spatio-topic) coordinates at early stages of visual processing. Most psychophysical studies that have attempted to examine this question have concluded that features are represented in retinal coordinates, but the interpretation of these experiments does not preclude the type of retinospatiotopic representation that is embodied in our model. The model also explains why electrical stimulation experiments in visual cortex cannot distinguish between retinal and retinospatiotopic coordinates in the early stages of visual processing. Psychophysical predictions are made for testing the existence of retinospatiotopic representations.

[1]  I. Kohler,et al.  The formation and transformation of the perceptual world. , 1963 .

[2]  L. Matin,et al.  Visual Perception of Direction for Stimuli Flashed During Voluntary Saccadic Eye Movements , 1965, Science.

[3]  C. McCollough,et al.  THE CONDITIONING OF COLOR-PERCEPTION. , 1965, The American journal of psychology.

[4]  P. Marchiafava,et al.  Modulation of transmission of optic nerve impulses in the alert cat: evidence of presynaptic inhibition of primary optic afferents during ocular movements. , 1966, Brain research.

[5]  D. M. MACKAY,et al.  Mislocation of Test Flashes during Saccadic Image Displacements , 1970, Nature.

[6]  J. Mayhew After-effects of movement contingent on direction of gaze. , 1973, Vision Research.

[7]  S. Mateeff Saccadic eye movements and localization of visual stimuli , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.

[8]  C Blakemore,et al.  Co‐ordination of head and eyes in the gaze changing behaviour of cats , 1980, The Journal of physiology.

[9]  O'Regan Jk,et al.  Integrating visual information from successive fixations: does trans-saccadic fusion exist? , 1983 .

[10]  Integrating visual information from successive fixations:Does trans-saccadic fusion exist? , 1983, Vision Research.

[11]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Object vision and spatial vision: two cortical pathways , 1983, Trends in Neurosciences.

[12]  K. Rayner,et al.  Is visual information integrated across saccades? , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[13]  I. Donaldson,et al.  Directional selectivity in the responses of units in cat primary visual cortex to passive eye movement , 1984, Neuroscience.

[14]  J. O'Regan,et al.  Retinal versus extraretinal influences in flash localization during saccadic eye movements in the presence of a visible background , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.

[15]  R. M. Siegel,et al.  Encoding of spatial location by posterior parietal neurons. , 1985, Science.

[16]  J. Feldman Four frames suffice: A provisional model of vision and space , 1985, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[17]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Learning internal representations by error propagation , 1986 .

[18]  David E. Irwin,et al.  Retinal masking during pursuit eye movements: implications for spatiotopic visual persistence. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[19]  R. Andersen,et al.  The role of the posterior parietal cortex in coordinate transformations for visual-motor integration. , 1988, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[20]  David E. Irwin,et al.  Visual masking and visual integration across saccadic eye movements. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[21]  J. Mcilwain Saccadic eye movements evoked by electrical stimulation of the cat's visual cortex , 1988, Visual Neuroscience.

[22]  Richard A. Andersen,et al.  A back-propagation programmed network that simulates response properties of a subset of posterior parietal neurons , 1988, Nature.

[23]  C. Galletti,et al.  Gaze-dependent visual neurons in area V3A of monkey prestriate cortex , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[24]  R. Lal,et al.  Gating of retinal transmission by afferent eye position and movement signals. , 1989, Science.

[25]  R. Andersen Visual and eye movement functions of the posterior parietal cortex. , 1989, Annual review of neuroscience.

[26]  Lawrence D. Jackel,et al.  Backpropagation Applied to Handwritten Zip Code Recognition , 1989, Neural Computation.

[27]  R. Andersen,et al.  Microstimulation of a Neural-Network Model for Visually Guided Saccades , 1989, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[28]  A Pollatsek,et al.  Role of spatial location in integration of pictorial information across saccades. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[29]  Richard A. Andersen,et al.  Algorithm programmed by a neural network model for coordinate transformation , 1990, 1990 IJCNN International Joint Conference on Neural Networks.

[30]  J. Vercher,et al.  The role of ocular muscle proprioception in visual localization of targets. , 1990, Science.

[31]  David E. Irwin,et al.  Visual memory and the perception of a stable visual environment , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[32]  K. Rayner,et al.  Role of spatial location in integration of pictorial information across saccades , 1990 .

[33]  S. R. Lehky,et al.  Neural models of binocular depth perception. , 1990, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.

[34]  David E. Irwin Information integration across saccadic eye movements , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.

[35]  D. J. Felleman,et al.  Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. , 1991, Cerebral cortex.

[36]  A. A. Skavenski,et al.  Eye movements elicited by electrical stimulation of area PG in the monkey. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[38]  S Thorpe,et al.  Modulation of neural stereoscopic processing in primate area V1 by the viewing distance. , 1992, Science.

[39]  Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al.  A distributed common reference frame for egocentric space in the posterior parietal cortex , 1992 .

[40]  Richard A. Andersen,et al.  Coordinate transformations in the representation of spatial information , 1993, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[41]  D. Alfred Owens,et al.  Seeing where we look: Fixation as extraretinal information , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[42]  M. Hayhoe,et al.  Task dependent spatial memory across saccades , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[43]  A. P. Petrov The “calibration” solution still leaves much work to be done , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[44]  D. E. Irwin,et al.  A localist evaluation solution for visual stability across saccades , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[45]  E. N. Sokolov,et al.  Vector code in space constancy , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[46]  Alexander H. Wertheim,et al.  Fixations or smooth eye movements? , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[47]  O. Grüsser,et al.  Early concepts on efference copy and reafference , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[48]  U. Windhorst,et al.  Visual stability: What is new? , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[49]  Eugene Chekaluk,et al.  Is there a role for extraretinal factors in the maintenance of stability in a structured environment? , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[50]  Horst Mittelstaedt,et al.  Theory of coordinate transformation by efference copy survives another attack , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[51]  J. T. Enright Voluntary oscillopsia: Watching the world go round , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[52]  S. Mateeff,et al.  Is there any essential difference between the “calibration” and “elimination” solutions? , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[53]  Bruce Bridgeman,et al.  A theory of visual stability across saccadic eye movements , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[54]  Michelle Fleury,et al.  The perceptual stability of the visual field: What is calibration for? , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[55]  W. Shebilske Calibration models and ecological efference mediation theory: Toward a synthesis of indirect and direct perception theories , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[56]  D. Robinson On the locus of visual stability , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[57]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  How our world remains stable despite disturbing influences , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[58]  L. Harris Keeping track of visual codes that move from cell to cell during eye movements , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[59]  Heiner Deubel,et al.  Perceptual stability and postsaccadic visual information: Can man bridge a gap? , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[60]  A. Stoper The translation solution plus motion suppression account for perceived stability , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[61]  M. Jüttner Visual stability and transsaccadic information processing , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[62]  J. Kevin O'Regan,et al.  The world as an outside iconic memory – no strong internal metric means no problem of visual stability , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[63]  E. Brenner,et al.  Stability relative to what? , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[64]  M. Schlag-Rey,et al.  Just how different are perceptual and visuomotor localization abilities? , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[65]  C. Prablanc Neuronal death of the cancellation theory? , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[66]  Arnold Trehub,et al.  What does calibration solve? , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[67]  Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al.  Is perception isomorphic with neural activity? , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[68]  K. J.,et al.  Retinal versus extraretinal influences in flash localization during saccadic eye movements in the presence of a visible background , 2022 .