Conditional discrimination and reversal in amnesia subsequent to hypoxic brain injury or anterior communicating artery aneurysm rupture

Human anterograde amnesia can develop following bilateral damage to the hippocampus and medial temporal lobes, as in hypoxic brain injury, or following damage to the basal forebrain, as following anterior communicating artery (ACoA) aneurysm rupture. In both cases, the mnestic deficit may be similar when assessed by standard neuropsychological measures. However, animal and computational models suggest that there are qualitative differences in the pattern of impaired and spared memory abilities following damage to hippocampus versus basal forebrain. Here, we show such a dissociation in human amnesia using a single two-stage task, involving conditional discrimination and reversal. Consistent with a prior study, 10 individuals with anterograde amnesia subsequent to hypoxic brain injury were spared on acquisition but impaired at reversal. However, 10 individuals with amnesia subsequent to ACoA aneurysm showed the opposite pattern of impaired acquisition but spared reversal. The differences between groups cannot be easily ascribed to severity of mnestic or cognitive deficit, since the two amnesic groups performed similarly on neuropsychological tests of memory, intelligence and attention. The results illustrate qualitative differences in memory impairments in hypoxic and ACoA amnesics and highlight the importance of considering etiology in evaluating mnemonic deficits in amnesic populations.

[1]  J. Theios,et al.  Acquisition and extinction of a classically conditioned response in hippocampectomized rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus). , 1972, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[2]  M. Mesulam,et al.  Trajectories of cholinergic pathways within the cerebral hemispheres of the human brain. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[3]  M. Freedman,et al.  Spatial and visual learning deficits in Alzheimer's and Parkinson's disease , 1989, Brain and Cognition.

[4]  M. Moscovitch,et al.  Memory for temporal order and conditional associative-learning in patients with Parkinson's disease , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  The possible contribution of the septal region to memory , 1997 .

[6]  Shelley Channon,et al.  T-Maze Discrimination and Reversal Learning After Unilateral Temporal or Frontal Lobe Lesions in Man , 1991, Cortex.

[7]  Catherine E. Myers,et al.  Further implications of a computational model of septohippocampal cholinergic modulation in eyeblink conditioning , 1998, Psychobiology.

[8]  P. Solomon,et al.  Altered activity in the hippocampus is more detrimental to classical conditioning than removing the structure. , 1983, Science.

[9]  M. Meunier,et al.  Cerebral ischemia: Are the memory deficits associated with hippocampal cell loss? , 1996, Hippocampus.

[10]  S. Phillips,et al.  Basal forebrain infarction. A clinicopathologic correlation. , 1987, Archives of neurology.

[11]  M. Gluck,et al.  Ibotenic acid lesions of the medial septum retard delay eyeblink conditioning in rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus). , 2002, Behavioral neuroscience.

[12]  Catherine E. Myers,et al.  Conditional spatial discrimination in humans with hypoxic brain injury , 2000, Psychobiology.

[13]  S. D. Berry,et al.  Medial septal lesions retard classical conditioning of the nicitating membrane response in rabbits. , 1979, Science.

[14]  Joseph R. Manns,et al.  Recognition Memory and the Human Hippocampus , 2003, Neuron.

[15]  J. DeLuca Predicting Neurobehavioral Patterns Following Anterior Communicating Artery Aneurysm , 1993, Cortex.

[16]  B. Milner,et al.  Deficits on subject-ordered tasks after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man , 1982, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  E. Warrington,et al.  Conditioning in amnesic patients , 1979, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  T. Robbins,et al.  Probabilistic learning and reversal deficits in patients with Parkinson’s disease or frontal or temporal lobe lesions: possible adverse effects of dopaminergic medication , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[19]  P. Solomon,et al.  Latent inhibition and stimulus generalization of the classically conditioned nictitating membrane response in rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus) following dorsal hippocampal ablation. , 1975, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[20]  H. Markowitsch,et al.  Human Memory Dysfunctions Due to Septal Lesions , 2000 .

[21]  M Freedman,et al.  Orbitofrontal function, object alternation and perseveration. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.

[22]  R P Kesner,et al.  Short-term memory for duration and distance in humans: role of the hippocampus. , 2001, Neuropsychology.

[23]  Andrew P. Smith,et al.  Memory impairment following ruptured aneurysm of the anterior communicating artery , 1988, Brain and Cognition.

[24]  Michael Petrides,et al.  Nonspatial conditional learning impaired in patients with unilateral frontal but not unilateral temporal lobe excisions , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[25]  M. Gluck,et al.  Impaired probabilistic category learning in hypoxic subjects with hippocampal damage , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[26]  M. Farah,et al.  Ventromedial frontal cortex mediates affective shifting in humans: evidence from a reversal learning paradigm. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[27]  T. Robbins,et al.  Central cholinergic systems and cognition. , 1997, Annual review of psychology.

[28]  Joong-Uhn Choi R. Numan (ed): The behavioral neuroscience of the septal region , 2001, Child's Nervous System.

[29]  M A Gluck,et al.  Impaired delay eyeblink classical conditioning in individuals with anterograde amnesia resulting from anterior communicating artery aneurysm rupture. , 2001, Behavioral neuroscience.

[30]  Ramona O Hopkins,et al.  Effects of hypoxia on the brain: Neuroimaging and neuropsychological findings following carbon monoxide poisoning and obstructive sleep apnea , 2004, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[31]  K M Heilman,et al.  Amnesia following a discrete basal forebrain lesion. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[32]  D. Woodruff-Pak,et al.  Eyeblink classical conditioning in H.M.: delay and trace paradigms. , 1993, Behavioral neuroscience.

[33]  Catherine E. Myers,et al.  Dissociating basal forebrain and medial temporal amnesic syndromes: Insights from classical conditioning , 2002, Integrative physiological and behavioral science : the official journal of the Pavlovian Society.

[34]  G. Goldenberg,et al.  Basal forebrain amnesia: does the nucleus accumbens contribute to human memory? , 1999, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[35]  J. DeLuca,et al.  Aneurysm of the anterior communicating artery: a review of neuroanatomical and neuropsychological sequelae. , 1995, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[36]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  A Computational Model of Cholinergic Disruption of Septohippocampal Activity in Classical Eyeblink Conditioning , 1996, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[37]  L. Weaver,et al.  Severe anoxia with and without concomitant brain atrophy and neuropsychological impairments , 1995, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[38]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  Laminar selectivity of the cholinergic suppression of synaptic transmission in rat hippocampal region CA1: computational modeling and brain slice physiology , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[39]  R. Ridley,et al.  Effects of lesions of different parts of the septo-hippocampal system in primates on learning and retention of information acquired before or after surgery , 1996, Brain Research Bulletin.

[40]  M. Gluck,et al.  A connectionist model of septohippocampal dynamics during conditioning: closing the loop. , 2002, Behavioral neuroscience.

[41]  Ramona O Hopkins,et al.  Semantic Memory and the Human Hippocampus , 2003, Neuron.

[42]  M. Petrides Deficits on conditional associative-learning tasks after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man , 1985, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  D. Amaral,et al.  Hippocampal abnormalities in amnesic patients revealed by high-resolution magnetic resonance imaging , 1989, Nature.

[44]  E. Rolls,et al.  Reward-related Reversal Learning after Surgical Excisions in Orbito-frontal or Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex in Humans , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[45]  M. Gluck,et al.  Context, conditioning, and hippocampal rerepresentation in animal learning. , 1994, Behavioral neuroscience.

[46]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  Spared discrimination and impaired reversal eyeblink conditioning in patients with temporal lobe amnesia. , 2001 .

[47]  Kazuo Abe,et al.  Amnesia after a discrete basal forebrain lesion , 1998, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[48]  B. Sahakian,et al.  Deficits in decision-making in patients with aneurysms of the anterior communicating artery. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[49]  M. Gluck,et al.  Intact delay-eyeblink classical conditioning in amnesia. , 1995, Behavioral neuroscience.

[50]  R. Numan The Behavioral Neuroscience of the Septal Region , 2012, Springer New York.

[51]  Mark A. Gluck,et al.  Ibotenic acid lesions of the medial septum retard delay eyeblink conditioning in rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus) , 2002 .

[52]  Adrian M. Owen,et al.  Cognitive Deficits in Brain Disorders , 2001 .

[53]  M. Gluck,et al.  Hippocampal mediation of stimulus representation: A computational theory , 1993, Hippocampus.

[54]  N. McNaughton,et al.  Comparison between the behavioural effects of septal and hippocampal lesions: A review , 1983, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.