Distinct Representations of Magnitude and Spatial Position within Parietal Cortex during Number–Space Mapping

Mapping numbers onto space is foundational to mathematical cognition. These cognitive operations are often conceptualized in the context of a “mental number line” and involve multiple brain regions in or near the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) that have been implicated both in numeral and spatial cognition. Here we examine possible differentiation of function within these brain areas in relating numbers to spatial positions. By isolating the planning phase of a number line task and introducing spatiotopic mapping tools from fMRI into mental number line task research, we are able to focus our analysis on the neural activity of areas in anterior IPS (aIPS) previously associated with number processing and on spatiotopically organized areas in and around posterior IPS (pIPS), while participants prepare to place a number on a number line. Our results support the view that the nonpositional magnitude of a numerical symbol is coded in aIPS, whereas the position of a number in space is coded in posterior areas of IPS. By focusing on the planning phase, we are able to isolate activation related to the cognitive, rather than the sensory–motor, aspects of the task. Also, to allow the separation of spatial position from magnitude, we tested both a standard positive number line (0 to 100) and a zero-centered mixed number line (−100 to 100). We found evidence of a functional dissociation between aIPS and pIPS: Activity in aIPS was associated with a landmark distance effect not modulated by spatial position, whereas activity in pIPS revealed a contralateral preference effect.

[1]  Nora S. Newcombe,et al.  Development of Quantitative Thinking Across Correlated Dimensions , 2016 .

[2]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .

[3]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Development of Elementary Numerical Abilities: A Neuronal Model , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[4]  E. Ramirez,et al.  Numerical Estimation in Children for Both Positive and Negative Numbers , 2016 .

[5]  B. Wandell,et al.  Visual Field Maps in Human Cortex , 2007, Neuron.

[6]  G. Denes,et al.  A specific deficit for numbers in a case of dense acalculia. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[7]  Bruce Fischl,et al.  Accurate and robust brain image alignment using boundary-based registration , 2009, NeuroImage.

[8]  Michael D. Dodd,et al.  Perceiving numbers causes spatial shifts of attention , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[9]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  How do we convert a number into a finger trajectory? , 2013, Cognition.

[10]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images , 2001, Medical Image Anal..

[11]  Hilary C Barth,et al.  The development of numerical estimation: evidence against a representational shift. , 2011, Developmental science.

[12]  Rafael Núñez,et al.  No Innate Number Line in the Human Brain , 2011 .

[13]  Geetha B. Ramani,et al.  Playing linear numerical board games promotes low-income children's numerical development. , 2008, Developmental science.

[14]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[15]  Liang Wang,et al.  Probabilistic Maps of Visual Topography in Human Cortex. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[16]  B. P. Klein,et al.  Topographic Representation of Numerosity in the Human Parietal Cortex , 2013, Science.

[17]  S. Dehaene,et al.  THREE PARIETAL CIRCUITS FOR NUMBER PROCESSING , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[18]  D. Somers,et al.  Functional MRI reveals spatially specific attentional modulation in human primary visual cortex. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[19]  Julie L. Booth,et al.  Developmental and individual differences in pure numerical estimation. , 2006, Developmental psychology.

[20]  Michael Brady,et al.  Improved Optimization for the Robust and Accurate Linear Registration and Motion Correction of Brain Images , 2002, NeuroImage.

[21]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[22]  David C. Geary,et al.  Development of Number Line Representations in Children With Mathematical Learning Disability , 2008, Developmental neuropsychology.

[23]  Torkel Klingberg,et al.  Specialization of the Right Intraparietal Sulcus for Processing Mathematics During Development , 2016, Cerebral cortex.

[24]  Denis G. Pelli,et al.  ECVP '07 Abstracts , 2007, Perception.

[25]  A. Schleicher,et al.  Cytoarchitectonic identification and probabilistic mapping of two distinct areas within the anterior ventral bank of the human intraparietal sulcus , 2006, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[26]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  Multiplexed Echo Planar Imaging for Sub-Second Whole Brain FMRI and Fast Diffusion Imaging , 2010, PloS one.

[27]  B. Argall,et al.  Simplified intersubject averaging on the cortical surface using SUMA , 2006, Human brain mapping.

[28]  Robert S. Siegler,et al.  An Integrative Theory of Numerical Development. , 2014 .

[29]  Robert S. Siegler,et al.  Overlapping and distinct brain regions involved in estimating the spatial position of numerical and non-numerical magnitudes: An fMRI study , 2013, Neuropsychologia.

[30]  Elida V. Laski,et al.  Is 27 a big number? Correlational and causal connections among numerical categorization, number line estimation, and numerical magnitude comparison. , 2007, Child development.

[31]  Korbinian Moeller,et al.  On the Relation between the Mental Number Line and Arithmetic Competencies , 2014, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[32]  M. Ashcraft,et al.  Cognitive processes of numerical estimation in children. , 2012, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[33]  Lotfi B Merabet,et al.  Visual Topography of Human Intraparietal Sulcus , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  A. Dale,et al.  Cortical Surface-Based Analysis II: Inflation, Flattening, and a Surface-Based Coordinate System , 1999, NeuroImage.

[35]  Julie L. Booth,et al.  Student Magnitude Knowledge of Negative Numbers , 2015, J. Numer. Cogn..

[36]  S. Kastner,et al.  Topographic maps in human frontal and parietal cortex , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[37]  Lucia Melloni,et al.  Motion along the mental number line reveals shared representations for numerosity and space , 2016, eLife.

[38]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  A validation of eye movements as a measure of elementary school children's developing number sense , 2008 .

[39]  K. Priftis,et al.  Brain damage: Neglect disrupts the mental number line , 2002, Nature.

[40]  B. Wandell,et al.  Visual field maps, population receptive field sizes, and visual field coverage in the human MT+ complex. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  Sabrina Brigadoi,et al.  On the Role of the Inferior Intraparietal Sulcus in Visual Working Memory for Lateralized Single-feature Objects , 2017, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[42]  Wim Fias,et al.  Common and distinct brain regions in both parietal and frontal cortex support symbolic and nonsymbolic number processing in humans: A functional neuroimaging meta-analysis , 2017, NeuroImage.

[43]  Wim Fias,et al.  Representation of Number in Animals and Humans: A Neural Model , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[44]  Dana Ganor-Stern,et al.  Negative numbers are generated in the mind. , 2008, Experimental psychology.

[45]  Julie L. Booth,et al.  Numerical magnitude representations influence arithmetic learning. , 2008, Child development.

[46]  Margot J. Taylor,et al.  Is 2+2=4? Meta-analyses of brain areas needed for numbers and calculations , 2011, NeuroImage.

[47]  Tom Verguts,et al.  Spatial Intuition in Elementary Arithmetic: A Neurocomputational Account , 2012, PloS one.

[48]  Mary Hegarty,et al.  Mathematical misunderstandings : qualitative reasoning about quantitative problems , 1992 .

[49]  H. Barth,et al.  Developmental change in numerical estimation. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[50]  Aiden E. G. F. Arnold,et al.  Structural connectivity of visuotopic intraparietal sulcus , 2013, NeuroImage.

[51]  R. Siegler,et al.  The Development of Numerical Estimation , 2003, Psychological science.

[52]  Benjamin D. Singer,et al.  Retinotopic Organization of Human Ventral Visual Cortex , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[53]  V. Michel,et al.  Recruitment of an Area Involved in Eye Movements During Mental Arithmetic , 2009, Science.

[54]  Vincent Walsh A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[55]  D. Heeger,et al.  Spatial attention affects brain activity in human primary visual cortex. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[56]  Stephen M Smith,et al.  Fast robust automated brain extraction , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[57]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Cultural Recycling of Cortical Maps , 2007, Neuron.

[58]  Anders M. Dale,et al.  Cortical Surface-Based Analysis I. Segmentation and Surface Reconstruction , 1999, NeuroImage.

[59]  Martin H. Fischer,et al.  Cognition in the bisection task , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[60]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Involvement of striate and extrastriate visual cortical areas in spatial attention , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.