Testing Multi-Alternative Decision Models with Non-Stationary Evidence

Recent research has investigated the process of integrating perceptual evidence toward a decision, converging on a number of sequential sampling choice models, such as variants of race and diffusion models and the non-linear leaky competing accumulator (LCA) model. Here we study extensions of these models to multi-alternative choice, considering how well they can account for data from a psychophysical experiment in which the evidence supporting each of the alternatives changes dynamically during the trial, in a way that creates temporal correlations. We find that participants exhibit a tendency to choose an alternative whose evidence profile is temporally anti-correlated with (or dissimilar from) that of other alternatives. This advantage of the anti-correlated alternative is well accounted for in the LCA, and provides constraints that challenge several other models of multi-alternative choice.

[1]  Dhanistha Panyasak,et al.  Circuits , 1995, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[2]  J. Schall,et al.  Neural Control of Voluntary Movement Initiation , 1996, Science.

[3]  J. Andel Sequential Analysis , 2022, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[4]  Donald Laming,et al.  Information theory of choice-reaction times , 1968 .

[5]  D. Vickers,et al.  Evidence for an accumulator model of psychophysical discrimination. , 1970, Ergonomics.

[6]  James L. McClelland,et al.  The time course of perceptual choice: the leaky, competing accumulator model. , 2001, Psychological review.

[7]  Marius Usher,et al.  Extending a biologically inspired model of choice: multi-alternatives, nonlinearity and value-based multidimensional choice , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[8]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Decision-making with multiple alternatives , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[9]  Miguel P Eckstein,et al.  The time course of visual information accrual guiding eye movement decisions. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[10]  W. Newsome,et al.  Neural basis of a perceptual decision in the parietal cortex (area LIP) of the rhesus monkey. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  J. Gold,et al.  Banburismus and the Brain Decoding the Relationship between Sensory Stimuli, Decisions, and Reward , 2002, Neuron.

[12]  W T Newsome,et al.  Target selection for saccadic eye movements: prelude activity in the superior colliculus during a direction-discrimination task. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  Xiao-Jing Wang,et al.  A Recurrent Network Mechanism of Time Integration in Perceptual Decisions , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  Sam Behseta,et al.  On the Effects of Signal Acuity in a Multi-Alternative Model of Decision Making , 2010, Neural Computation.

[15]  Timothy D. Hanks,et al.  Bounded Integration in Parietal Cortex Underlies Decisions Even When Viewing Duration Is Dictated by the Environment , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Modeling reaction time and accuracy of multiple-alternative decisions , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[17]  Xiao-Jing Wang,et al.  Similarity Effect and Optimal Control of Multiple-Choice Decision Making , 2008, Neuron.

[18]  Jochen Ditterich,et al.  Perceptual Decisions between Multiple Directions of Visual Motion , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  P. Holmes,et al.  The dynamics of choice among multiple alternatives , 2006 .

[20]  J. Townsend,et al.  Multialternative Decision Field Theory: A Dynamic Connectionist Model of Decision Making , 2001 .

[21]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Response of Neurons in the Lateral Intraparietal Area during a Combined Visual Discrimination Reaction Time Task , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Integration of Sensory and Reward Information during Perceptual Decision-Making in Lateral Intraparietal Cortex (LIP) of the Macaque Monkey , 2010, PloS one.

[23]  J. Gold,et al.  The neural basis of decision making. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[24]  Xiao-Jing Wang,et al.  Probabilistic Decision Making by Slow Reverberation in Cortical Circuits , 2002, Neuron.

[25]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Loss aversion and inhibition in dynamical models of multialternative choice. , 2004, Psychological review.

[26]  A. Tversky Elimination by aspects: A theory of choice. , 1972 .

[27]  Jochen Ditterich,et al.  A Comparison between Mechanisms of Multi-Alternative Perceptual Decision Making: Ability to Explain Human Behavior, Predictions for Neurophysiology, and Relationship with Decision Theory , 2010, Front. Neurosci..

[28]  M. Stone Models for choice-reaction time , 1960 .

[29]  J. Gold,et al.  Neural computations that underlie decisions about sensory stimuli , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[30]  Rafal Bogacz,et al.  A comparison of bounded diffusion models for choice in time controlled tasks. , 2009, Journal of mathematical psychology.

[31]  Ian Krajbich,et al.  Visual fixations and the computation and comparison of value in simple choice , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[32]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  Modeling Response Times for Two-Choice Decisions , 1998 .

[33]  Andrew Heathcote,et al.  A ballistic model of choice response time. , 2005, Psychological review.

[34]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  Dual diffusion model for single-cell recording data from the superior colliculus in a brightness-discrimination task. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  Kevin N. Gurney,et al.  The Basal Ganglia and Cortex Implement Optimal Decision Making Between Alternative Actions , 2007, Neural Computation.

[36]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .

[37]  S. Link,et al.  A sequential theory of psychological discrimination , 1975 .

[38]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  A general mechanism for perceptual decision-making in the human brain , 2004, Nature.

[39]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Neural Activity in Macaque Parietal Cortex Reflects Temporal Integration of Visual Motion Signals during Perceptual Decision Making , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[40]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  A comparison of sequential sampling models for two-choice reaction time. , 2004, Psychological review.

[41]  Casimir J. H. Ludwig,et al.  The Temporal Impulse Response Underlying Saccadic Decisions , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[42]  Rafal Bogacz,et al.  Optimal decision-making theories , 2009 .

[43]  N. Chater,et al.  Preference reversal in multiattribute choice. , 2010, Psychological review.

[44]  M. Shadlen,et al.  A role for neural integrators in perceptual decision making. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[45]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The physics of optimal decision making: a formal analysis of models of performance in two-alternative forced-choice tasks. , 2006, Psychological review.

[46]  Xiao-Jing Wang Decision Making in Recurrent Neuronal Circuits , 2008, Neuron.

[47]  A. Raftery Bayesian Model Selection in Social Research , 1995 .

[48]  A. Tversky,et al.  Loss Aversion in Riskless Choice: A Reference-Dependent Model , 1991 .

[49]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  The simplest complete model of choice response time: Linear ballistic accumulation , 2008, Cognitive Psychology.

[50]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  The Diffusion Decision Model: Theory and Data for Two-Choice Decision Tasks , 2008, Neural Computation.

[51]  R. Ratcliff Modeling response signal and response time data , 2006, Cognitive Psychology.