The Ferrier lecture, 1980

The main factors limiting the performance of the peripheral parts of the visual system can be specified, and doing this clarifies the nature of the interpretive tasks that must be performed by the central parts of the system. It is argued that the critical factor that hinders development of better resolving power is the difficulty of confining light within the waveguide-like outer segment, and that for sensitivity this critical factor is the thermal decomposition of photosensitive pigments. Knowledge of these limits makes many surprising details of the eye intelligible. Understanding the difficulties posed by the narrow dynamic range of nerve fibres may give similar insight into the coding of the retinal image for transmission to the brain. Our level of understanding changes when we come to the visual cortex, for although we do not lack good anatomical and neurophysiological data, these do not make the principles of operation self-evident in the way that the structure of the eye immediately suggests that it is an image-forming device. The cortex converts the representation of the visual field that it receives into reliable knowledge of the world around us, and the trouble may be that we lack good models of how this can be done. A system that can respond to single quanta and resolve almost to the diffraction limit is unlikely to employ grossly inefficient methods for those higher functions upon which its whole utility depends, and so it is worth seeking out the limiting factors. The quality of human performance at certain higher perceptual tasks is high compared with the limit of reliable statistical inference; hence much of the sample of information available in a visual image must be effectively utilized. But there are strong limitations on the connectivity in the cortex, so that one is forced to consider how the relevant information can be collected together. Three stages of dealing with the visual image are proposed: the improvement of the cortical map in primary visual cortex by processes analogous to spatial and temporal interpolation ; the detection of linking features in this map; and the concentration of this information by non-topographical mapping in adjacent visual areas.

[1]  E. Andersen,et al.  VISUAL PERCEPTION AND THE RETINAL MOSAIC , 1923 .

[2]  K. J. Craik The effect of adaptation on differential brightness discrimination , 1938, The Journal of physiology.

[3]  G. L. Walls POSTSCRIPT ON IMAGE EXPANSION BY THE FOVEAL CLIVUS , 1940 .

[4]  S. Hecht,et al.  ENERGY, QUANTA, AND VISION , 1942, The Journal of general physiology.

[5]  G. L. Walls,et al.  The Vertebrate Eye and Its Adaptive Radiation. , 2013 .

[6]  W. D. Wright,et al.  Colour Sensitivity of the Fovea Centralis , 1945, Nature.

[7]  K. Chow,et al.  Cell ratios in the thalamo‐cortical visual system of Macaca mulatta , 1950, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[8]  H. Barlow The Size of Ommatidia in Apposition Eyes , 1952 .

[9]  H. Barlow Summation and inhibition in the frog's retina , 1953, The Journal of physiology.

[10]  S. W. Kuffler Discharge patterns and functional organization of mammalian retina. , 1953, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  H. Barlow Retinal noise and absolute threshold. , 1956, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[12]  H. Barlow Purkinje Shift and Retinal Noise , 1957, Nature.

[13]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields of single neurones in the cat's striate cortex , 1959, The Journal of physiology.

[14]  W. Pitts,et al.  What the Frog's Eye Tells the Frog's Brain , 1959, Proceedings of the IRE.

[15]  U. T. Keesey Effects of involuntary eye movements on visual acuity. , 1960, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[16]  J. Platt How we see straight Lines. , 1960, Scientific American.

[17]  D. Whitteridge,et al.  The representation of the visual field on the cerebral cortex in monkeys , 1961, The Journal of physiology.

[18]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex , 1962, The Journal of physiology.

[19]  H. Barlow Chapter 16 – THE PHYSICAL LIMITS OF VISUAL DISCRIMINATION , 1964 .

[20]  M. Land Image formation by a concave reflector in the eye of the scallop, Pecten maximus. , 1965, The Journal of physiology.

[21]  H. Barlow,et al.  The mechanism of directionally selective units in rabbit's retina. , 1965, The Journal of physiology.

[22]  C. Blakemore,et al.  The neural mechanism of binocular depth discrimination , 1967, The Journal of physiology.

[23]  Adolfo Guzman,et al.  Decomposition of a visual scene into three-dimensional bodies , 1968 .

[24]  D. Hubel,et al.  The period of susceptibility to the physiological effects of unilateral eye closure in kittens , 1970, The Journal of physiology.

[25]  D. Hubel,et al.  Stereoscopic Vision in Macaque Monkey: Cells sensitive to Binocular Depth in Area 18 of the Macaque Monkey Cortex , 1970, Nature.

[26]  B. Julesz Foundations of Cyclopean Perception , 1971 .

[27]  H. Barlow,et al.  Responses to single quanta of light in retinal ganglion cells of the cat. , 1971, Vision research.

[28]  R. Shlaer An Eagle's Eye: Quality of the Retinal Image , 1972, Science.

[29]  A. Snyder Coupled-Mode Theory for Optical Fibers , 1972 .

[30]  D. Hubel,et al.  Laminar and columnar distribution of geniculo‐cortical fibers in the macaque monkey , 1972, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[31]  T. Powell,et al.  Patterns of degeneration after intrinsic lesions of the visual cortex (area 17) of the monkey. , 1973, Brain research.

[32]  V. Braitenberg Gehirngespinste: Neuroanatomie für kybernetisch Interessierte , 1973 .

[33]  S. Zeki Functional organization of a visual area in the posterior bank of the superior temporal sulcus of the rhesus monkey , 1974, The Journal of physiology.

[34]  JOHN Ross,et al.  Stereopsis by binocular delay , 1974, Nature.

[35]  D. Hubel,et al.  Ordered arrangement of orientation columns in monkeys lacking visual experience , 1974, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[36]  G. Horridge Optical mechanisms of clear-zone eyes , 1974 .

[37]  J. Kaas,et al.  A crescent-shaped cortical visual area surrounding the middle temporal area (MT) in the owl monkey (Aotus trivirgatus). , 1974, Brain research.

[38]  J. Ross,et al.  The pulfrich effect and short-term memory in stereopsis , 1975, Vision Research.

[39]  M. Morgan,et al.  Apparent Motion and the Pulfrich Effect , 1975, Perception.

[40]  C. Gilbert,et al.  The projections of cells in different layers of the cat's visual cortex , 1975, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[41]  MICHAEL MORGAN,et al.  Stereoillusion based on visual persistence , 1975, Nature.

[42]  S. McKee,et al.  Visual acuity in the presence of retinal-image motion. , 1975, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[43]  P. O. Bishop,et al.  Direction selectivity of simple striate cells: properties and mechanism. , 1975, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  R. Fox,et al.  Falcon visual acuity. , 1976, Science.

[45]  J. Kaas,et al.  Representation of the visual field on the medial wall of occipital-parietal cortex in the owl monkey. , 1976, Science.

[46]  M. Morgan Pulfrich Effect and the Filling in of Apparent Motion , 1976, Perception.

[47]  M. Land Superposition images are formed by reflection in the eyes of some oceanic decapod Crustacea , 1976, Nature.

[48]  C. Gilbert,et al.  Laminar patterns of geniculocortical projection in the cat , 1976, Brain Research.

[49]  W. H. Miller,et al.  Photoreceptor diameter and spacing for highest resolving power. , 1977, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[50]  G. Poggio,et al.  Binocular interaction and depth sensitivity in striate and prestriate cortex of behaving rhesus monkey. , 1977, Journal of neurophysiology.

[51]  C. R. Michael Color vision mechanisms in monkey striate cortex: simple cells with dual opponent-color receptive fields. , 1978, Journal of neurophysiology.

[52]  C. Enroth-Cugell,et al.  Light distribution in the cat's retinal image , 1978, Vision Research.

[53]  W. H. Miller,et al.  Telephoto lens system of falconiform eyes , 1978, Nature.

[54]  C. R. Michael Color-sensitive complex cells in monkey striate cortex. , 1978, Journal of neurophysiology.

[55]  R. Blake Spatial Vision in the Cat , 1978 .

[56]  S. Zeki Functional specialisation in the visual cortex of the rhesus monkey , 1978, Nature.

[57]  M. Morgan,et al.  Smooth eye tracking and the perception of motion in the absence of real movement , 1978, Vision Research.

[58]  H. Barlow The efficiency of detecting changes of density in random dot patterns , 1978, Vision Research.

[59]  H. Barlow,et al.  The versatility and absolute efficiency of detecting mirror symmetry in random dot displays , 1979, Vision Research.

[60]  M. Imbert Maturation of Visual Cortex with and without Visual Experience , 1979 .

[61]  D. Burr Acuity for apparent vernier offset , 1979, Vision Research.

[62]  D. Baylor,et al.  Responses of retinal rods to single photons. , 1979, The Journal of physiology.

[63]  D. Baylor,et al.  Thermal activation of the visual transduction mechanism in retinal rods , 1979, Nature.

[64]  A. Cowey Cortical Maps and Visual Perception the Grindley Memorial Lecture* , 1979, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[65]  H. Wässle,et al.  Size, scatter and coverage of ganglion cell receptive field centres in the cat retina. , 1979, The Journal of physiology.

[66]  David C. Burr,et al.  How does binocular delay give information about depth? , 1979, Vision Research.

[67]  A. B. Bonds Development of Orientation Tuning in the Visual Cortex of Kittens , 1979 .

[68]  M. Land,et al.  Compound eyes: old and new optical mechanisms , 1980, Nature.

[69]  J. Maunsell,et al.  Two‐dimensional maps of the cerebral cortex , 1980, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[70]  G. Horridge Review lecture: Apposition eyes of large diurnal insects as organs adapted to seeing , 1980, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[71]  L. Palmer,et al.  Retinotopic organization of areas 20 and 21 in the cat , 1980, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[72]  S. Zeki The representation of colours in the cerebral cortex , 1980, Nature.

[73]  D. Marr,et al.  An Information Processing Approach to Understanding the Visual Cortex , 1980 .

[74]  J. E. Albano,et al.  Visual-motor function of the primate superior colliculus. , 1980, Annual review of neuroscience.

[75]  A. Sillito,et al.  A re-evaluation of the mechanisms underlying simple cell orientation selectivity , 1980, Brain Research.

[76]  R. Freeman,et al.  Monocularly deprived humans: nondeprived eye has supernormal vernier acuity. , 1980, Journal of neurophysiology.

[77]  V. Montero Patterns of connections from the striate cortex to cortical visual areas in superior temporal sulcus of macaque and middle temporal gyrus of owl monkey , 1980, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[78]  H. Barlow The absolute efficiency of perceptual decisions. , 1980, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[79]  W. Singer,et al.  The effects of early visual experience on the cat's visual cortex and their possible explanation by Hebb synapses. , 1981, The Journal of physiology.

[80]  D. Burr Temporal summation of moving images by the human visual system , 1981, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[81]  H. Barlow,et al.  The statistical efficiency for detecting sinusoidal modulation of average dot density in random figures , 1981, Vision Research.

[82]  B. Boycott,et al.  Morphology and mosaic of on- and off-beta cells in the cat retina and some functional considerations , 1981, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[83]  A. Fuchs,et al.  The development of spatial‐frequency selectivity in kitten striate cortex. , 1981, The Journal of physiology.

[84]  D. Ferster A comparison of binocular depth mechanisms in areas 17 and 18 of the cat visual cortex , 1981, The Journal of physiology.