The Roles of the Caudate Nucleus in Human Classification Learning

The caudate nucleus is commonly active when learning relationships between stimuli and responses or categories. Previous research has not differentiated between the contributions to learning in the caudate and its contributions to executive functions such as feedback processing. We used event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging while participants learned to categorize visual stimuli as predicting “rain” or “sun.” In each trial, participants viewed a stimulus, indicated their prediction via a button press, and then received feedback. Conditions were defined on the bases of stimulus-outcome contingency (deterministic, probabilistic, and random) and feedback (negative and positive). A region of interest analysis was used to examine activity in the head of the caudate, body/tail of the caudate, and putamen. Activity associated with successful learning was localized in the body and tail of the caudate and putamen; this activity increased as the stimulus-outcome contingencies were learned. In contrast, activity in the head of the caudate and ventral striatum was associated most strongly with processing feedback and decreased across trials. The left superior frontal gyrus was more active for deterministic than probabilistic stimuli; conversely, extrastriate visual areas were more active for probabilistic than deterministic stimuli. Overall, hippocampal activity was associated with receiving positive feedback but not with correct classification. Successful learning correlated positively with activity in the body and tail of the caudate nucleus and negatively with activity in the hippocampus.

[1]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[2]  D. Stuss,et al.  Cognitive neuroscience. , 1993, Current opinion in neurobiology.

[3]  J. Hogg Magnetic resonance imaging. , 1994, Journal of the Royal Naval Medical Service.

[4]  Carol A. Seger,et al.  Implicit Learning , 2019 .

[5]  M. Gluck,et al.  Probabilistic classification learning in amnesia. , 1994, Learning & memory.

[6]  D. Gaffan,et al.  Visual Learning for an Auditory Secondary Reinforcer by Macaques is Intact after Uncinate Fascicle Section: Indirect Evidence for the Involvement of the Corpus Striatum , 1995, The European journal of neuroscience.

[7]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Responses of cells in the tail of the caudate nucleus during visual discrimination learning. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  L R Squire,et al.  Dissociable properties of memory systems: differences in the flexibility of declarative and nondeclarative knowledge. , 1996, Behavioral neuroscience.

[9]  Jane S. Paulsen,et al.  Dissociations within nondeclarative memory in Huntington's disease. , 1996 .

[10]  Jennifer A. Mangels,et al.  A Neostriatal Habit Learning System in Humans , 1996, Science.

[11]  C. Gerfen,et al.  The frontal cortex-basal ganglia system in primates. , 1996, Critical reviews in neurobiology.

[12]  J. Mink THE BASAL GANGLIA: FOCUSED SELECTION AND INHIBITION OF COMPETING MOTOR PROGRAMS , 1996, Progress in Neurobiology.

[13]  P. Strick,et al.  The temporal lobe is a target of output from the basal ganglia. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[14]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Differential Activation of the Caudate Nucleus in Primates Performing Spatial and Nonspatial Working Memory Tasks , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  Gregory Ashby,et al.  A neuropsychological theory of multiple systems in category learning. , 1998, Psychological review.

[16]  L R Squire,et al.  A reexamination of the concurrent discrimination learning task: the importance of anterior inferotemporal cortex, area TE. , 1998, Behavioral neuroscience.

[17]  T. Robbins,et al.  Cognitive functions and corticostriatal circuits: insights from Huntington's disease , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[18]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Alternative strategies of categorization , 1998, Cognition.

[19]  P. Reber,et al.  Contrasting cortical activity associated with category memory and recognition memory. , 1998, Learning & memory.

[20]  P J Reber,et al.  Cortical areas supporting category learning identified using functional MRI. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[21]  J. Hollerman,et al.  Modifications of reward expectation-related neuronal activity during learning in primate striatum. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  L. Squire,et al.  A reexamination of the concurrent discrimination learning task , 1998 .

[23]  Carol A. Seger,et al.  Striatal activation during acquisition of a cognitive skill. , 1999, Neuropsychology.

[24]  Seth J. Ramus,et al.  Dissociation between the effects of damage to perirhinal cortex and area TE. , 1999, Learning & memory.

[25]  Ivan Toni,et al.  Prefrontal-basal ganglia pathways are involved in the learning of arbitrary visuomotor associations: a PET study , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  A M Dale,et al.  Optimal experimental design for event‐related fMRI , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[27]  L R Squire,et al.  Intact learning of artificial grammars and intact category learning by patients with Parkinson's disease. , 1999, Behavioral neuroscience.

[28]  C. Carter,et al.  Complementary Category Learning Systems Identified Using Event-Related Functional MRI , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[29]  Russell A Poldrack,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetries and individual differences in visual concept learning as measured by functional MRI , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[30]  T. Robbins,et al.  Contrasting Cortical and Subcortical Activations Produced by Attentional-Set Shifting and Reversal Learning in Humans , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[31]  S. Wise,et al.  Arbitrary associations between antecedents and actions , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.

[32]  L. Squire,et al.  Contrasting Effects on Discrimination Learning after Hippocampal Lesions and Conjoint Hippocampal–Caudate Lesions in Monkeys , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  Nikolaus R. McFarland,et al.  Striatonigrostriatal Pathways in Primates Form an Ascending Spiral from the Shell to the Dorsolateral Striatum , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  Samuel M. McClure,et al.  Predictability Modulates Human Brain Response to Reward , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Visual habit formation in monkeys with neurotoxic lesions of the ventrocaudal neostriatum , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[36]  W. T. Maddox,et al.  Striatal contributions to category learning: Quantitative modeling of simple linear and complex nonlinear rule learning in patients with Parkinson's disease , 2001, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[37]  R. Passingham,et al.  Learning Arbitrary Visuomotor Associations: Temporal Dynamic of Brain Activity , 2001, NeuroImage.

[38]  M. Petrides,et al.  Wisconsin Card Sorting Revisited: Distinct Neural Circuits Participating in Different Stages of the Task Identified by Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  M. Gluck,et al.  Interactive memory systems in the human brain , 2001, Nature.

[40]  Gregory S. Berns,et al.  The Context of Uncertainty Modulates the Subcortical Response to Predictability , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[41]  P. Apicella,et al.  Reward Unpredictability inside and outside of a Task Context as a Determinant of the Responses of Tonically Active Neurons in the Monkey Striatum , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[42]  W T Maddox,et al.  A possible role of the striatum in linear and nonlinear category learning: evidence from patients with Huntington's disease. , 2001, Behavioral neuroscience.

[43]  G. Glover,et al.  Spiral‐in/out BOLD fMRI for increased SNR and reduced susceptibility artifacts , 2001, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[44]  R. Passingham,et al.  Neural correlates of visuomotor associations. Spatial rules compared with arbitrary rules. , 2001, Experimental brain research.

[45]  K. Witt,et al.  Dissociation of Habit-Learning in Parkinson's and Cerebellar Disease , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[46]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  A neural correlate of response bias in monkey caudate nucleus , 2002, Nature.

[47]  B. Knowlton,et al.  Learning and memory functions of the Basal Ganglia. , 2002, Annual review of neuroscience.

[48]  R. Passingham,et al.  Changes of cortico-striatal effective connectivity during visuomotor learning. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[49]  Corey J Bohil,et al.  Observational versus feedback training in rule-based and information-integration category learning , 2002, Memory & cognition.

[50]  S. Kéri,et al.  Probabilistic classification learning in Tourette syndrome , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[51]  Sándor Beniczky,et al.  Dissociation between Attentional Set Shifting and Habit Learning: a Longitudinal Case Study , 2002, Neurocase.

[52]  Patrick Dupont,et al.  Human Brain Regions Involved in Visual Categorization , 2002, NeuroImage.

[53]  T. Robbins,et al.  Defining the Neural Mechanisms of Probabilistic Reversal Learning Using Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[54]  Corey J. Bohil,et al.  Delayed feedback effects on rule-based and information-integration category learning. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[55]  Barbara J Knowlton,et al.  Analysis of probabilistic classification learning in patients with Parkinson's disease before and after pallidotomy surgery. , 2003, Learning & memory.

[56]  Paul J. Reber,et al.  Dissociating Explicit and Implicit Category Knowledge with fMRI , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[57]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Competition among multiple memory systems: converging evidence from animal and human brain studies , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[58]  Samuel M. McClure,et al.  Temporal Prediction Errors in a Passive Learning Task Activate Human Striatum , 2003, Neuron.

[59]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Temporal Difference Models and Reward-Related Learning in the Human Brain , 2003, Neuron.

[60]  W. Schultz,et al.  Discrete Coding of Reward Probability and Uncertainty by Dopamine Neurons , 2003, Science.

[61]  M. Gluck,et al.  Dissociating Hippocampal versus Basal Ganglia Contributions to Learning and Transfer , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[62]  F. Gregory Ashby,et al.  The cognitive neuroscience of implicit category learning , 2003 .

[63]  L. Jiménez Attention and implicit learning , 2003 .

[64]  D. Yves von Cramon,et al.  Predicting events of varying probability: uncertainty investigated by fMRI , 2003, NeuroImage.

[65]  M. Gluck,et al.  Human midbrain sensitivity to cognitive feedback and uncertainty during classification learning. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[66]  M. Petrides,et al.  Neural Bases of Set-Shifting Deficits in Parkinson's Disease , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[67]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dissociable Roles of Ventral and Dorsal Striatum in Instrumental Conditioning , 2004, Science.

[68]  M. Gluck,et al.  Impaired probabilistic category learning in hypoxic subjects with hippocampal damage , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[69]  Samuel M. McClure,et al.  Separate Neural Systems Value Immediate and Delayed Monetary Rewards , 2004, Science.

[70]  M. Delgado,et al.  Motivation-dependent responses in the human caudate nucleus. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[71]  M. Gluck,et al.  Cortico-striatal contributions to feedback-based learning: converging data from neuroimaging and neuropsychology. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[72]  T. Robbins,et al.  Differential Responses in Human Striatum and Prefrontal Cortex to Changes in Object and Rule Relevance , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[73]  Alison R Preston,et al.  Comparison of spiral-in/out and spiral-out BOLD fMRI at 1.5 and 3 T , 2004, NeuroImage.

[74]  K. Doya,et al.  A Neural Correlate of Reward-Based Behavioral Learning in Caudate Nucleus: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of a Stochastic Decision Task , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[75]  M. Delgado,et al.  Modulation of Caudate Activity by Action Contingency , 2004, Neuron.

[76]  Joan Y. Chiao,et al.  An Event-related fMRI Study of Artificial Grammar Learning in a Balanced Chunk Strength Design , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[77]  W. T. Maddox,et al.  Cortical and subcortical brain regions involved in rule-based category learning , 2005, Neuroreport.

[78]  W Todd Maddox,et al.  Information-integration category learning in patients with striatal dysfunction. , 2005, Neuropsychology.