Cortico-striatal contributions to feedback-based learning: converging data from neuroimaging and neuropsychology.

The striatum has been widely implicated in cognition, but a precise understanding of its role remains elusive. Here we present converging evidence for the role of the striatum in feedback-based learning. In a prior functional imaging study, healthy controls showed striatal activity during a feedback-based learning task, which was decreased when the same task was learned without feedback. In the present study, we show that individuals with striatal dysfunction due to Parkinson's disease are impaired on the feedback-based task, but not on a non-feedback version of the same task. Parkinson's patients and controls also used different learning strategies depending on feedback structure. This study provides direct behavioural evidence from humans that cortico-striatal systems are necessary for feedback-based learning on a cognitive task. These findings also link between learning impairments in Parkinson's disease and the physiological and computational evidence for the role of midbrain dopaminergic systems in feedback processing.

[1]  M. Hoehn,et al.  Parkinsonism , 1967, Neurology.

[2]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[3]  J. Saint-Cyr,et al.  Procedural learning and neostriatal dysfunction in man. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[4]  D. Salmon,et al.  Neuropsychological evidence for multiple implicit memory systems: a comparison of Alzheimer's, Huntington's, and Parkinson's disease patients , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[5]  A. C. Roberts,et al.  Impaired extra-dimensional shift performance in medicated and unmedicated Parkinson's disease: Evidence for a specific attentional dysfunction , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  M. Moscovitch,et al.  Memory for temporal order and conditional associative-learning in patients with Parkinson's disease , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  M. Bondi,et al.  Implicit and explicit memory in Alzheimer's disease and Parkinson's disease. , 1991, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[8]  W. Schultz,et al.  Responses of monkey dopamine neurons during learning of behavioral reactions. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  P. Calabresi,et al.  Long-term synaptic depression in the striatum: physiological and pharmacological characterization , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[10]  L. Squire Declarative and Nondeclarative Memory: Multiple Brain Systems Supporting Learning and Memory , 1992, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[11]  T. Robbins,et al.  Contrasting mechanisms of impaired attentional set-shifting in patients with frontal lobe damage or Parkinson's disease. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[12]  C. Cepeda,et al.  Neuromodulatory actions of dopamine in the neostriatum are dependent upon the excitatory amino acid receptor subtypes activated. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[13]  M. Gluck,et al.  Probabilistic classification learning in amnesia. , 1994, Learning & memory.

[14]  T. Robbins,et al.  Refining the Taxonomy of Memory , 1996, Science.

[15]  J. Wickens,et al.  Dopamine reverses the depression of rat corticostriatal synapses which normally follows high-frequency stimulation of cortex In vitro , 1996, Neuroscience.

[16]  L. Squire,et al.  Structure and function of declarative and nondeclarative memory systems. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[17]  Jennifer A. Mangels,et al.  A Neostriatal Habit Learning System in Humans , 1996, Science.

[18]  Gurindar S. Sohi,et al.  Memory systems , 1996, CSUR.

[19]  M. Koivisto,et al.  Perceptual priming in Alzheimer's and Parkinson's diseases , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  Peter Dayan,et al.  A Neural Substrate of Prediction and Reward , 1997, Science.

[21]  J. Gabrieli Cognitive neuroscience of human memory. , 1998, Annual review of psychology.

[22]  J. Houk,et al.  Model of cortical-basal ganglionic processing: encoding the serial order of sensory events. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  Intact learning of artificial grammars and intact category learning by patients with Parkinson's disease. , 1999 .

[24]  C. I. Connolly,et al.  Building neural representations of habits. , 1999, Science.

[25]  R. Poldrack Imaging Brain Plasticity: Conceptual and Methodological Issues— A Theoretical Review , 2000, NeuroImage.

[26]  J. Hollerman,et al.  Involvement of basal ganglia and orbitofrontal cortex in goal-directed behavior. , 2000, Progress in brain research.

[27]  T. Robbins,et al.  Probabilistic learning and reversal deficits in patients with Parkinson’s disease or frontal or temporal lobe lesions: possible adverse effects of dopaminergic medication , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  J. Horvitz Mesolimbocortical and nigrostriatal dopamine responses to salient non-reward events , 2000, Neuroscience.

[29]  J. Wickens,et al.  Substantia nigra dopamine regulates synaptic plasticity and membrane potential fluctuations in the rat neostriatum, in vivo , 2000, Neuroscience.

[30]  John McDowall,et al.  Preserved Implicit Learning on Both the Serial Reaction Time Task and Artificial Grammar in Patients with Parkinson's Disease , 2001, Brain and Cognition.

[31]  N. Cohen From Conditioning to Conscious Recollection Memory Systems of the Brain. Oxford Psychology Series, Volume 35. , 2001 .

[32]  M. Gluck,et al.  Interactive memory systems in the human brain , 2001, Nature.

[33]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  From Conditioning to Conscious Recollection , 2001 .

[34]  T. Robbins,et al.  Enhanced or impaired cognitive function in Parkinson's disease as a function of dopaminergic medication and task demands. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[35]  G Deuschl,et al.  Intact artificial grammar learning in patients with cerebellar degeneration and advanced Parkinson’s disease , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  W. Schultz Getting Formal with Dopamine and Reward , 2002, Neuron.

[37]  M. Gluck,et al.  How do people solve the "weather prediction" task?: individual variability in strategies for probabilistic category learning. , 2002, Learning & memory.

[38]  M. Gluck,et al.  Dissociating Hippocampal versus Basal Ganglia Contributions to Learning and Transfer , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.