Examining the role of the temporo-parietal network in memory, imagery, and viewpoint transformations

The traditional view of the medial temporal lobe (MTL) focuses on its role in episodic memory. However, some of the underlying functions of the MTL can be ascertained from its wider role in supporting spatial cognition in concert with parietal and prefrontal regions. The MTL is strongly implicated in the formation of enduring allocentric representations (e.g., O'Keefe, 1976; King et al., 2002; Ekstrom et al., 2003). According to our BBB model (Byrne et al., 2007), these representations must interact with head-centered and body-centered representations in posterior parietal cortex via a transformation circuit involving retrosplenial areas. Egocentric sensory representations in parietal areas can then cue the recall of allocentric spatial representations in long-term memory and, conversely, the products of retrieval in MTL can generate mental imagery within a parietal “window.” Such imagery is necessarily egocentric and forms part of visuospatial working memory, in which it can be manipulated for the purpose of planning/imagining the future. Recent fMRI evidence (Lambrey et al., 2012; Zhang et al., 2012) supports the BBB model. To further test the model, we had participants learn the locations of objects in a virtual scene and tested their spatial memory under conditions that impose varying demands on the transformation circuit. We analyzed how brain activity correlated with accuracy in judging the direction of an object (1) from visuospatial working memory (we assume transient working memory due to the order of tasks and the absence of change in viewpoint, but long-term memory retrieval is also possible), (2) after a rotation of viewpoint, or (3) after a rotation and translation of viewpoint (judgment of relative direction). We found performance-related activity in both tasks requiring viewpoint rotation (ROT and JRD, i.e., conditions 2 and 3) in the core medial temporal to medial parietal circuit identified by the BBB model. These results are consistent with the predictions of the BBB model, and shed further light on the neural mechanisms underlying spatial memory, mental imagery and viewpoint transformations.

[1]  Katherine R. Sherrill,et al.  Hippocampus and Retrosplenial Cortex Combine Path Integration Signals for Successful Navigation , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  Arne D. Ekstrom,et al.  Human neural systems underlying rigid and flexible forms of allocentric spatial representation , 2013, Human brain mapping.

[3]  Alain Berthoz,et al.  Selective role of lingual/parahippocampal gyrus and retrosplenial complex in spatial memory across viewpoint changes relative to the environmental reference frame , 2013, Behavioural Brain Research.

[4]  Arne D. Ekstrom,et al.  Differential Recruitment of Brain Networks following Route and Cartographic Map Learning of Spatial Environments , 2012, PloS one.

[5]  V. Bohbot,et al.  Eye tracking, strategies, and sex differences in virtual navigation , 2012, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[6]  Jessica A. Grahn,et al.  The cognitive functions of the caudate nucleus , 2008, Progress in Neurobiology.

[7]  Rüdiger Wenzel,et al.  Proprioceptive head posture-related processing in human polysensory cortical areas , 2008, NeuroImage.

[8]  G. Taguchi,et al.  Fundamentals of Data Analysis , 2007 .

[9]  Mingsha Zhang,et al.  The proprioceptive representation of eye position in monkey primary somatosensory cortex , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[10]  S. Becker,et al.  Remembering the past and imagining the future: a neural model of spatial memory and imagery. , 2007, Psychological review.

[11]  E. Maguire,et al.  Navigation around London by a taxi driver with bilateral hippocampal lesions. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[12]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  Analysis of functional image analysis contest (FIAC) data with brainvoyager QX: From single‐subject to cortically aligned group general linear model analysis and self‐organizing group independent component analysis , 2006, Human brain mapping.

[13]  Michael C. Wendl,et al.  Argonaute—a database for gene regulation by mammalian microRNAs , 2005, BMC Bioinformatics.

[14]  Luigi Landini,et al.  Advanced Image Processing in Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2005 .

[15]  S. Platek,et al.  Common Ground for Spatial Cognition? A Behavioral and fMRI Study of Sex Differences in Mental Rotation and Spatial Working Memory , 2005 .

[16]  J. Galen Buckwalter,et al.  Sex differences in mental rotation and spatial rotation in a virtual environment , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  Neil Burgess,et al.  Orientational manoeuvres in the dark: dissociating allocentric and egocentric influences on spatial memory , 2004, Cognition.

[18]  M. May,et al.  Imaginal perspective switches in remembered environments: Transformation versus interference accounts , 2004, Cognitive Psychology.

[19]  K. Jeffery,et al.  Heterogeneous Modulation of Place Cell Firing by Changes in Context , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  Arne D. Ekstrom,et al.  Cellular networks underlying human spatial navigation , 2003, Nature.

[21]  Bradley R. Postle,et al.  Spatial working memory activity of the caudate nucleus is sensitive to frame of reference , 2003, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[22]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Thresholding of Statistical Maps in Functional Neuroimaging Using the False Discovery Rate , 2002, NeuroImage.

[23]  E. Maguire The retrosplenial contribution to human navigation: a review of lesion and neuroimaging findings. , 2001, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[24]  Morris Moscovitch,et al.  Remote spatial memory in an amnesic person with extensive bilateral hippocampal lesions , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[25]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Topographical disorientation: a synthesis and taxonomy. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[26]  L. Nadel,et al.  Spatial memory deficits in patients with lesions to the right hippocampus and to the right parahippocampal cortex , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[27]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  A cortical representation of the local visual environment , 1998, Nature.

[28]  N. Takahashi,et al.  Pure topographic disorientation due to right retrosplenial lesion , 1997, Neurology.

[29]  T. McNamara,et al.  Multiple views of spatial memory , 1997 .

[30]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Improved Assessment of Significant Activation in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI): Use of a Cluster‐Size Threshold , 1995, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[31]  Susan D. Voyer,et al.  Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: a meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. , 1995, Psychological bulletin.

[32]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[33]  J. Rieser Access to Knowledge of Spatial Structure at Novel Points of Observation , 1989 .

[34]  A. Sirigu,et al.  Pure Topographical Disorientation: A Definition and Anatomical Basis , 1987, Cortex.

[35]  J. O’Keefe Place units in the hippocampus of the freely moving rat , 1976, Experimental Neurology.

[36]  Christian F. Doeller,et al.  Imagining being somewhere else: neural basis of changing perspective in space. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[37]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  Fundamentals of data analysis methods in fMRI , 2006 .

[38]  Ranxiao Frances Wang,et al.  Beyond imagination: Perspective change problems revisited , 2005 .

[39]  D. Amaral,et al.  Macaque monkey retrosplenial cortex: II. Cortical afferents , 2003, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[40]  John A. King,et al.  Human hippocampus and viewpoint dependence in spatial memory , 2002, Hippocampus.

[41]  N. Cohen From Conditioning to Conscious Recollection Memory Systems of the Brain. Oxford Psychology Series, Volume 35. , 2001 .

[42]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  From Conditioning to Conscious Recollection , 2001 .

[43]  J. Michael Wyass,et al.  Connections between the retrosplenial cortex and the hippocampal formation in the rat: A review , 1992, Hippocampus.

[44]  J. Rieser Access to knowledge of spatial structure at novel points of observation. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[45]  J. Jaspars,et al.  Cognitive functions. , 1966, Annual review of psychology.

[46]  J. Müller,et al.  Of the brain. , 1837 .

[47]  Analysis of FIAC data with BrainVoyager QX : From single subject to cortically aligned group GLM analysis , 2022 .