Transitivity, flexibility, conjunctive representations, and the hippocampus. II. A computational analysis

A computational neural network model is presented that explains how the hippocampus can contribute to transitive inference performance observed in rats (Dusek and Eichenbaum, 1997. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 94:7109–7114; Van Elzakker et al., 2003. Hippocampus 12:this issue). In contrast to existing theories that emphasize the idea that the hippocampus contributes by flexibly relating previously encoded memories, we find that the hippocampus contributes by altering the elemental associative weights of individual stimulus elements during learning. We use this model to account for a range of existing data and to make a number of distinctive predictions that clearly contrast these two views. Hippocampus 2003;13:341–354. © 2003 Wiley‐Liss, Inc.

[1]  L. Kamin Attention-like processes in classical conditioning , 1967 .

[2]  D Marr,et al.  Simple memory: a theory for archicortex. , 1971, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[3]  R. Rescorla,et al.  A theory of Pavlovian conditioning : Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement , 1972 .

[4]  T. Trabasso,et al.  On the construction and use of representations involving linear order , 1975 .

[5]  Alan S. Brown,et al.  Information Processing and Cognition: The Loyola Symposium , 1976 .

[6]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Hippocampal synaptic enhancement and information storage within a distributed memory system , 1987, Trends in Neurosciences.

[7]  R. Sutherland,et al.  Configural association theory: The role of the hippocampal formation in learning, memory, and amnesia , 1989, Psychobiology.

[8]  E. Rolls Functions of neuronal networks in the hippocampus and neocortex in memory , 1989 .

[9]  Thomas R. Zentall,et al.  Transitive inference in pigeons , 1991 .

[10]  Michael Davis,et al.  The role of the amygdala in conditioned fear. , 1992 .

[11]  M. Fanselow,et al.  Modality-specific retrograde amnesia of fear. , 1992, Science.

[12]  E. Lauterbach The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction , 1993 .

[13]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Hippocampal conjunctive encoding, storage, and recall: Avoiding a trade‐off , 1994, Hippocampus.

[14]  M. Hasselmo Neuromodulation and cortical function: modeling the physiological basis of behavior , 1995, Behavioural Brain Research.

[15]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. , 1995, Psychological review.

[16]  R. Sutherland,et al.  Configural association theory and the hippocampal formation: An appraisal and reconfiguration , 1995, Hippocampus.

[17]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  Conservation of hippocampal memory function in rats and humans , 1996, Nature.

[18]  Randall C. O'Reilly,et al.  Biologically Plausible Error-Driven Learning Using Local Activation Differences: The Generalized Recirculation Algorithm , 1996, Neural Computation.

[19]  M. Fanselow,et al.  Neurotoxic lesions of the dorsal hippocampus and Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats , 1997, Behavioural Brain Research.

[20]  Xiangbao Wu,et al.  A simple, biologically motivated neural network solves the transitive inference problem , 1997, Proceedings of International Conference on Neural Networks (ICNN'97).

[21]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A Hippocampal Model of Recognition Memory , 1997, NIPS.

[22]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  The hippocampus and memory for orderly stimulus relations. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[23]  Martina Siemann Juan D. Delius Algebraic Learning and Neural Network Models for Transitive and Non-transitive Responding , 1998 .

[24]  Alcino J. Silva,et al.  The dorsal hippocampus is essential for context discrimination but not for contextual conditioning. , 1998, Behavioral neuroscience.

[25]  R. O’Reilly Six principles for biologically based computational models of cortical cognition , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[26]  H Eichenbaum,et al.  The hippocampus and transverse patterning guided by olfactory cues. , 1998, Behavioral neuroscience.

[27]  M. Fanselow,et al.  Temporally Graded Retrograde Amnesia of Contextual Fear after Hippocampal Damage in Rats: Within-Subjects Examination , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  J. Rawlins,et al.  Dissociating context and space within the hippocampus: effects of complete, dorsal, and ventral excitotoxic hippocampal lesions on conditioned freezing and spatial learning. , 1999, Behavioral neuroscience.

[29]  R. O’Reilly,et al.  Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience: Understanding the Mind by Simulating the Brain , 2000 .

[30]  William B. Levy,et al.  Simulating symbolic distance effects in the transitive inference problem , 2001, Neurocomputing.

[31]  R. O’Reilly,et al.  Conjunctive representations, the hippocampus, and contextual fear conditioning , 2001, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[32]  R. O’Reilly,et al.  Conjunctive representations in learning and memory: principles of cortical and hippocampal function. , 2001, Psychological review.

[33]  R. O’Reilly,et al.  Hippocampal formation supports conditioning to memory of a context. , 2002, Behavioral neuroscience.

[34]  Michael Van Elzakker,et al.  Transitivity, flexibility, conjunctive representations, and the hippocampus. I. An empirical analysis , 2003, Hippocampus.