When Do Objects Become Landmarks? A VR Study of the Effect of Task Relevance on Spatial Memory

We investigated how objects come to serve as landmarks in spatial memory, and more specifically how they form part of an allocentric cognitive map. Participants performing a virtual driving task incidentally learned the layout of a virtual town and locations of objects in that town. They were subsequently tested on their spatial and recognition memory for the objects. To assess whether the objects were encoded allocentrically we examined pointing consistency across tested viewpoints. In three experiments, we found that spatial memory for objects at navigationally relevant locations was more consistent across tested viewpoints, particularly when participants had more limited experience of the environment. When participants’ attention was focused on the appearance of objects, the navigational relevance effect was eliminated, whereas when their attention was focused on objects’ locations, this effect was enhanced, supporting the hypothesis that when objects are processed in the service of navigation, rather than merely being viewed as objects, they engage qualitatively distinct attentional systems and are incorporated into an allocentric spatial representation. The results are consistent with evidence from the neuroimaging literature that when objects are relevant to navigation, they not only engage the ventral “object processing stream”, but also the dorsal stream and medial temporal lobe memory system classically associated with allocentric spatial memory.

[1]  R. Muller,et al.  On the directional firing properties of hippocampal place cells , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[2]  Pierre Lavenex,et al.  Spatial relational learning and memory abilities do not differ between men and women in a real-world, open-field environment , 2010, Behavioural Brain Research.

[3]  S. Becker,et al.  Remembering the past and imagining the future: a neural model of spatial memory and imagery. , 2007, Psychological review.

[4]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[5]  B. McNaughton,et al.  The contributions of position, direction, and velocity to single unit activity in the hippocampus of freely-moving rats , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[6]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[7]  Cornelis G. Weststeijn,et al.  Neural representation of object location and route direction: An event-related fMRI study , 2007, Brain Research.

[8]  J. O’Keefe,et al.  Phase relationship between hippocampal place units and the EEG theta rhythm , 1993, Hippocampus.

[9]  Arne D. Ekstrom,et al.  Cellular networks underlying human spatial navigation , 2003, Nature.

[10]  T. McNamara,et al.  Different mental representations for place recognition and goal localization , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[11]  Neil Burgess,et al.  Distinct error-correcting and incidental learning of location relative to landmarks and boundaries , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[12]  R. Mansfield,et al.  Analysis of visual behavior , 1982 .

[13]  E. Spelke,et al.  Updating egocentric representations in human navigation , 2000, Cognition.

[14]  Christine M Valiquette,et al.  Locomotion, incidental learning, and the selection of spatial reference systems. , 2003, Memory & cognition.

[15]  Weimin Mou,et al.  Reference directions and reference objects in spatial memory of a briefly viewed layout , 2008, Cognition.

[16]  T. Schenk,et al.  An allocentric rather than perceptual deficit in patient D.F. , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[17]  G W Evans,et al.  Cognitive mapping: knowledge of real-world distance and location information. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[18]  E. Maguire,et al.  Knowing Where Things Are: Parahippocampal Involvement in Encoding Object Locations in Virtual Large-Scale Space , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  Giuseppe Iaria,et al.  Gray Matter Differences Correlate with Spontaneous Strategies in a Human Virtual Navigation Task , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  Neil Burgess,et al.  The hippocampal role in spatial memory and the familiarity--recollection distinction: a case study. , 2004, Neuropsychology.

[21]  Weimin Mou,et al.  Roles of egocentric and allocentric spatial representations in locomotion and reorientation. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[22]  L. Carlson,et al.  Selecting landmarks in novel environments , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[23]  E. Rolls,et al.  View-invariant representations of familiar objects by neurons in the inferior temporal visual cortex. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.

[24]  Weimin Mou,et al.  Allocentric and egocentric updating of spatial memories. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[25]  A. Berthoz,et al.  The neural basis of egocentric and allocentric coding of space in humans: a functional magnetic resonance study , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  T. McNamara,et al.  Multiple views of spatial memory , 1997 .

[27]  T. McNamara,et al.  Orientation and perspective dependence in route and survey learning. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[28]  Timothy P. McNamara,et al.  Systems of Spatial Reference in Human Memory , 2001, Cognitive Psychology.

[29]  John A. King,et al.  Human hippocampus and viewpoint dependence in spatial memory , 2002, Hippocampus.

[30]  M. Petrides,et al.  Cognitive Strategies Dependent on the Hippocampus and Caudate Nucleus in Human Navigation: Variability and Change with Practice , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  Jonathan W. Kelly,et al.  Spatial memories of virtual environments: How egocentric experience, intrinsic structure, and extrinsic structure interact , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[32]  Hans-Jochen Heinze,et al.  Different cortical activations for subjects using allocentric or egocentric strategies in a virtual navigation task , 2004, Neuroreport.

[33]  Russell A. Epstein,et al.  Neural correlates of real-world route learning , 2010, NeuroImage.

[34]  M. Chun,et al.  Contextual Cueing: Implicit Learning and Memory of Visual Context Guides Spatial Attention , 1998, Cognitive Psychology.

[35]  Dwight J. Kravitz,et al.  A new neural framework for visuospatial processing , 2011, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[36]  T. McNamara,et al.  Mental representations of large and small spatial layouts are orientation dependent. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[37]  M. Masson,et al.  Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[38]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[39]  M. Goodale,et al.  Frames of Reference for Perception and Action in the Human Visual System , 1998, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[40]  B. Schönebeck,et al.  The neural basis of ego- and allocentric reference frames in spatial navigation: Evidence from spatio-temporal coupled current density reconstruction , 2006, Brain Research.

[41]  E. Maguire,et al.  The Well-Worn Route and the Path Less Traveled Distinct Neural Bases of Route Following and Wayfinding in Humans , 2003, Neuron.

[42]  Bruce L. McNaughton,et al.  Place cell firing shows an inertia-like process , 2000, Neurocomputing.

[43]  Leslie G. Ungerleider Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .

[44]  Gabriele Janzen,et al.  Selective neural representation of objects relevant for navigation , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[45]  Michael J Kahana,et al.  A sense of direction in human entorhinal cortex , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[46]  T. McNamara,et al.  Intrinsic frames of reference in spatial memory. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[47]  Steffen Werner,et al.  Environmental reference systems for large-scale spaces , 1999, Spatial Cogn. Comput..

[48]  J. O’Keefe Place units in the hippocampus of the freely moving rat , 1976, Experimental Neurology.

[49]  Weimin Mou,et al.  Layout geometry in encoding and retrieval of spatial memory. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[50]  N. Burgess,et al.  Spatial memory: how egocentric and allocentric combine , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[51]  T. McNamara,et al.  Layout geometry in the selection of intrinsic frames of reference from multiple viewpoints. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[52]  C. Hölscher,et al.  Quinolinic acid lesion of the rat entorhinal cortex pars medialis produces selective amnesia in allocentric working memory (WM), but not in egocentric WM , 1994, Behavioural Brain Research.

[53]  Timothy P. McNamara,et al.  Locomotion, incidental learning, and the selection , 2003 .

[54]  Russell A. Epstein,et al.  Distances between Real-World Locations Are Represented in the Human Hippocampus , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[55]  Christine M Valiquette,et al.  Biased representations of the spatial structure of navigable environments , 2007, Psychological research.

[56]  K. Nakamura,et al.  Monkey hippocampal neurons related to spatial and nonspatial functions. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.