Impaired dimensional selection but intact use of reward feedback during visual discrimination learning in Parkinson's disease

It has been suggested that Parkinson's disease (PD) impairs the ability to learn on the basis of reward or reinforcing feedback i.e., by trial-and-error. In many learning tasks, particular 'dimensions' of stimulus information are relevant whilst others are irrelevant; therefore, efficient performance depends on identifying the dimensions of these 'compound' stimuli and selecting the relevant dimension for further processing. We investigated the ability of patients with PD, as well as patients with Huntington's disease and patients with frontal or temporal lobe lesions, to learn visual discriminations which required either a number of associations to be learned concurrently (the 'eight-pair' task) or the selection of information from compound stimuli (the 'five-dimension' task), both tasks being learned by trial-and-error. None of the basal ganglia disorder patient groups was impaired on the eight-pair task, militating against a crucial role for these brain structures in trial-and-error learning per se. Patients with mild, medicated PD, but not unmedicated PD patients, were impaired at identifying all five feature dimensions in the five-dimension task, implying dopaminergic 'overdosing' of the ability to analyse compound stimuli in terms of their component dimensions. Temporal lobe lesion patients performed similarly, suggesting that the temporal lobe may be the site of the medication overdose effect. Patients with severe, medicated PD were impaired at compound discrimination learning on the five-dimension task in the absence of an underlying impairment in identifying component stimulus dimensions; this pattern resembled that seen in Huntington's disease and frontal lobe lesion patients, implying that fronto-striatal circuitry is involved in the formation of rules based upon selected stimulus dimensions.

[1]  A. M. Owen,et al.  Visuospatial memory deficits at different stages of Parkinson's disease , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  S J Kish,et al.  Biochemical pathophysiology of Parkinson's disease. , 1987, Advances in neurology.

[3]  S. Iversen,et al.  Handbook of Psychopharmacology , 1988, Springer US.

[4]  T. Robbins,et al.  A comparative study of visuospatial memory and learning in Alzheimer-type dementia and Parkinson's disease. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[5]  M. Gluck,et al.  l-dopa impairs learning, but spares generalization, in Parkinson's disease , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  T. Robbins,et al.  Contrasting mechanisms of impaired attentional set-shifting in patients with frontal lobe damage or Parkinson's disease. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[7]  T. Robbins,et al.  l-Dopa medication remediates cognitive inflexibility, but increases impulsivity in patients with Parkinson’s disease , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. , 1986, Annual review of neuroscience.

[9]  M. Moscovitch,et al.  Memory for temporal order and conditional associative-learning in patients with Parkinson's disease , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  T. Robbins,et al.  Enhanced or impaired cognitive function in Parkinson's disease as a function of dopaminergic medication and task demands. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[11]  Jane S. Paulsen,et al.  Unified Huntington's disease rating scale: Reliability and consistency , 1996, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[12]  T. Robbins,et al.  Rule-abstraction Deficits Following a Basal Ganglia Lesion , 2001, Neurocase.

[13]  A. Lees,et al.  Cognitive deficits in the early stages of Parkinson's disease. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[14]  T. Robbins,et al.  Visual object and visuospatial cognition in Huntington's disease: implications for information processing in corticostriatal circuits. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[15]  T. Robbins,et al.  Probabilistic learning and reversal deficits in patients with Parkinson’s disease or frontal or temporal lobe lesions: possible adverse effects of dopaminergic medication , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  Jennifer A. Mangels,et al.  A Neostriatal Habit Learning System in Humans , 1996, Science.

[17]  B. Berger,et al.  Regional and laminar distribution of the dopamine and serotonin innervation in the macaque cerebral cortex: A radioautographic study , 1988, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[18]  F. Tamaru Disturbances in higher function in Parkinson's disease. , 1997, European neurology.

[19]  P. Strick,et al.  The temporal lobe is a target of output from the basal ganglia. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[20]  M. Horstink,et al.  Cognitive and motor shifting aptitude disorder in Parkinson's disease. , 1984, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[21]  S. Kish,et al.  Uneven pattern of dopamine loss in the striatum of patients with idiopathic Parkinson's disease. Pathophysiologic and clinical implications. , 1988, The New England journal of medicine.

[22]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[23]  D. Joel,et al.  Open interconnected model of basal ganglia‐thalamocortical circuitry and its relevance to the clinical syndrome of Huntington's disease , 2001, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[24]  W. Schultz Dopamine neurons and their role in reward mechanisms , 1997, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[25]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Dynamic Dopamine Modulation in the Basal Ganglia: A Neurocomputational Account of Cognitive Deficits in Medicated and Nonmedicated Parkinsonism , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  N. J. Slamecka A methodological analysis of shift paradigms in human discrimination learning. , 1968, Psychological bulletin.

[27]  Dean C. Delis,et al.  Componential analysis of problem-solving ability: Performance of patients with frontal lobe damage and amnesic patients on a new sorting test , 1992, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  Chris Rorden,et al.  Spatial Normalization of Brain Images with Focal Lesions Using Cost Function Masking , 2001, NeuroImage.

[29]  A. C. Roberts,et al.  Impaired extra-dimensional shift performance in medicated and unmedicated Parkinson's disease: Evidence for a specific attentional dysfunction , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[30]  M. Hoehn,et al.  Parkinsonism , 1967, Neurology.

[31]  R. Cools Dopaminergic modulation of cognitive function-implications for l-DOPA treatment in Parkinson's disease , 2006, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[32]  M. Gluck,et al.  Cortico-striatal contributions to feedback-based learning: converging data from neuroimaging and neuropsychology. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[33]  G. Chazot,et al.  Clinical and electrophysiological phenotype of a homozygously duplicated Charcot-Marie-Tooth (type 1A) disease. , 1997, European neurology.

[34]  C. Marsden,et al.  'Frontal' cognitive function in patients with Parkinson's disease 'on' and 'off' levodopa. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[35]  R. Beninger,et al.  Impaired Incentive Learning in Treated Parkinson’s Disease , 1996, Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences / Journal Canadien des Sciences Neurologiques.

[36]  C. Marsden,et al.  Fronto-striatal cognitive deficits at different stages of Parkinson's disease. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[37]  J. Aggleton,et al.  Concurrent and Sequential Pattern Discrimination Learning by Patients with Korsakoff Amnesia , 1990, Cortex.

[38]  C. Polkey Surgical alternatives for the treatment of temporal lobe epilepsy. , 1988, British journal of neurosurgery.

[39]  C. Rorden,et al.  Stereotaxic display of brain lesions. , 2000, Behavioural neurology.

[40]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  By Carrot or by Stick: Cognitive Reinforcement Learning in Parkinsonism , 2004, Science.

[41]  H. Braak,et al.  Staging of brain pathology related to sporadic Parkinson’s disease , 2003, Neurobiology of Aging.

[42]  L. Squire Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. , 1992, Psychological review.

[43]  Adrian M. Owen,et al.  Visuo-spatial short-term recognition memory and learning after temporal lobe excisions, frontal lobe excisions or amygdalo-hippocampectomy in man , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[44]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[45]  A. Beck,et al.  An inventory for measuring depression. , 1961, Archives of general psychiatry.

[46]  N. Mackintosh,et al.  Mechanisms of animal discrimination learning , 1971 .

[47]  Joseph B. Martin,et al.  Patterns of cell loss in Huntington's disease , 1987, Trends in Neurosciences.

[48]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Overlap of dopaminergic, adrenergic, and serotoninergic receptors and complementarity of their subtypes in primate prefrontal cortex , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.