Dynamic Cue Combination in Distributional Population Code Networks

In this chapter, we investigate how hierarchical networks of neural populations might code stable representations of transient stimulus variables along with the uncertainty about them, while allowing consistent semantics of inputs and outputs at every level of the neural hierarchy. One effective paradigm to study this question is the integration of information from multiple sensory sources. Significant behavioral and neurophysiological evidence substantiates the notion that when separate sensorymodalities receive correlated inputs about an external stimulus, the information is often combined to produce a coherent, unitary percept of the world (Stein & Meredith, 1993). This ability of neural systems to combine information from multiple sources affords considerable, ecologically relevant advantages. For instance, it has been shown to attenuate the effects of input variability owing to noise, thereby reducing the overall uncertainty in the final estimate of the stimulus variable. Sensory cue integration and sensorimotor combinations involve many important and challenging statistical issues; for example, the different sources of information are not equally reliable, and their reliability can change with changing stimulus conditions and task demands. Bayesian probability theory has provided a normative framework for predicting how sensory systems might integrate information optimally, to make perceptual inferences about the environment from noisy sensory data. These predictions have been quantitatively tested using psychophysical experiments that examine what computations are performed when reliability of the cues changes over time. Results from widely ranging studies of human cue integration are consistent in that subjects behave in a manner that takes the uncertainty in sensory inputs into account, to form statistically optimal estimates of stimulus variables (see Knill & Pouget, 2004, for a review). Several different computational strategies have been proposed andvalidated; some examples are linearweighted averaging (Hillis, Watt, Landy, & Banks, 2004; Jacobs, 1999; Knill & Saunders, 2003; van Beers, Sittig, & van der Gon, 1999), extensions thereof (Landy, Maloney, Johnston, & Young, 1995), multiplicative interactions, and fully probabilistic linear and nonlinear Bayesian inference (Knill, 2003). Regardless of the exact inference strategy, the common theme in all these studies is that sensory uncertainty determines the optimal weighting scheme for combining sensory inputs for perceptual judgments. Similarly, studies also demonstrate that sensory and motor uncertainties determine how sensory signals should be transformed and used to plan actions and guide behavior. An example of a commonly used paradigm involves manipulating continuous visual feedback from the hand to control pointingmovements. Subjects are able to compensate for experimentally induced changes

[1]  T. Flash,et al.  The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed mathematical model , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[2]  Frances S. Chance,et al.  Gain Modulation from Background Synaptic Input , 2002, Neuron.

[3]  Alexandre Pouget,et al.  Probabilistic Interpretation of Population Codes , 1996, Neural Computation.

[4]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Neuronal population coding of movement direction. , 1986, Science.

[5]  Rajesh P. N. Rao,et al.  Probabilistic Models of the Brain: Perception and Neural Function , 2002 .

[6]  A. Pouget,et al.  Reading population codes: a neural implementation of ideal observers , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[7]  Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al.  Neuronal Tuning: To Sharpen or Broaden? , 1999, Neural Computation.

[8]  Rajesh P. N. Rao Bayesian Computation in Recurrent Neural Circuits , 2004, Neural Computation.

[9]  Michael S Landy,et al.  Combining Priors and Noisy Visual Cues in a Rapid Pointing Task , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  H Sompolinsky,et al.  Simple models for reading neuronal population codes. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[11]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Spiking Boltzmann Machines , 1999, NIPS.

[12]  L F Abbott,et al.  Transfer of coded information from sensory to motor networks , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[13]  M. Paradiso,et al.  A theory for the use of visual orientation information which exploits the columnar structure of striate cortex , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[14]  A. Pouget,et al.  Efficient computation and cue integration with noisy population codes , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  M. Ernst,et al.  Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.

[16]  James M. Hillis,et al.  Combining Sensory Information: Mandatory Fusion Within, but Not Between, Senses , 2002, Science.

[17]  E N Brown,et al.  A Statistical Paradigm for Neural Spike Train Decoding Applied to Position Prediction from Ensemble Firing Patterns of Rat Hippocampal Place Cells , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  Geoffrey E. Hinton Products of experts , 1999 .

[19]  Rufin van Rullen,et al.  Rate Coding Versus Temporal Order Coding: What the Retinal Ganglion Cells Tell the Visual Cortex , 2001, Neural Computation.

[20]  B. Stein,et al.  The Merging of the Senses , 1993 .

[21]  Wei Ji Ma,et al.  Bayesian inference with probabilistic population codes , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[22]  Peter E. Latham,et al.  Narrow Versus Wide Tuning Curves: What's Best for a Population Code? , 1999, Neural Computation.

[23]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning , 2004, Nature.

[24]  James M. Hillis,et al.  Slant from texture and disparity cues: optimal cue combination. , 2004, Journal of vision.

[25]  P. Morasso Spatial control of arm movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  H Markram,et al.  Dynamics of population rate codes in ensembles of neocortical neurons. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  Nicolas Brunel,et al.  Mutual Information, Fisher Information, and Population Coding , 1998, Neural Computation.

[28]  R. Zemel,et al.  Inference and computation with population codes. , 2003, Annual review of neuroscience.

[29]  E Guigon,et al.  Recoding arm position to learn visuomotor transformations. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[30]  J. Saunders,et al.  Humans use continuous visual feedback from the hand to control fast reaching movements , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[31]  J. Gold,et al.  Neural computations that underlie decisions about sensory stimuli , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[32]  Jan J. Koenderink,et al.  Information in channel-coded systems: correlated receivers , 1992, Biological Cybernetics.

[33]  Matthew A. Wilson,et al.  Dynamic Analyses of Information Encoding in Neural Ensembles , 2004, Neural Computation.

[34]  A. Zador,et al.  Neural representation and the cortical code. , 2000, Annual review of neuroscience.

[35]  Peter Dayan,et al.  Encoding and Decoding Spikes for Dynamic Stimuli , 2008, Neural Computation.

[36]  Peter Dayan,et al.  Fast Population Coding , 2007, Neural Computation.

[37]  M. Landy,et al.  Measurement and modeling of depth cue combination: in defense of weak fusion , 1995, Vision Research.

[38]  J. J. Denier van der Gon,et al.  Integration of proprioceptive and visual position-information , 1999 .

[39]  David C Knill,et al.  Visual Feedback Control of Hand Movements , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[40]  Charles H. Anderson,et al.  BASIC ELEMENTS OF BIOLOGICAL COMPUTATIONAL SYSTEMS , 1994 .

[41]  Anthony J. Movshon,et al.  Optimal representation of sensory information by neural populations , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[42]  D. Knill,et al.  The Bayesian brain: the role of uncertainty in neural coding and computation , 2004, Trends in Neurosciences.

[43]  Peter Dayan,et al.  Combining Probabilistic Population Codes , 1997, IJCAI.

[44]  Bartlett W. Mel,et al.  Arithmetic of Subthreshold Synaptic Summation in a Model CA1 Pyramidal Cell , 2003, Neuron.

[45]  H. Barlow Summation and inhibition in the frog's retina , 1953, The Journal of physiology.

[46]  R. Jacobs,et al.  Optimal integration of texture and motion cues to depth , 1999, Vision Research.

[47]  Peter E. Latham,et al.  Statistically Efficient Estimation Using Population Coding , 1998, Neural Computation.

[48]  L. Abbott,et al.  A model of multiplicative neural responses in parietal cortex. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[49]  J. Saunders,et al.  Do humans optimally integrate stereo and texture information for judgments of surface slant? , 2003, Vision Research.

[50]  David C Knill,et al.  Mixture models and the probabilistic structure of depth cues , 2003, Vision Research.