Further implications of a computational model of septohippocampal cholinergic modulation in eyeblink conditioning

Previously we have shown that Gluck and Myers’s (1993) corticohippocampal model could be extended to incorporate Hasselmo and Schnell’s (1994) hypothesis that septohippocampal cholinergic processes regulate the amount of information storage in hippocampus. The generalized model could account for the effect of the anticholinergic drug scopolamine in delaying onset of eyeblink conditioning (Myers et al., 1996). Here, we show that the model also accounts for additional eyeblink results, including quick recovery after scopolamine is removed, preserved latent inhibition, learned irrelevance and extinction under scopolamine, and no effect of systemic scopolamine after hippocampal lesion. Additionally, the model is consistent with data concerning localized scopolamine injections to the medial septum, the lateral septum, and the hippocampus and their effect on eyeblink conditioning.

[1]  W. L. Milligan,et al.  Lateral septal lesions enhance conditioned bradycardia in the rabbit. , 1982, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[2]  M. Gluck,et al.  Cortico-hippocampal representations in simultaneous odor discrimination: a computational interpretation of Eichenbaum, Mathews, and Cohen (1989). , 1996, Behavioral neuroscience.

[3]  M. Fanselow,et al.  Scopolamine Selectively Disrupts the Acquisition of Contextual Fear Conditioning in Rats , 1995, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[4]  D. Woodruff-Pak,et al.  A nicotinic agonist (GTS-21), eyeblink classical conditioning and nicotinic receptor binding in rabbit brain , 1994, Brain Research.

[5]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  Dynamics of learning and recall at excitatory recurrent synapses and cholinergic modulation in rat hippocampal region CA3 , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[6]  J L Lewis,et al.  Lesions of the middle cerebellar peduncle disrupt acquisition and retention of the rabbit's classically conditioned nictitating membrane response. , 1987, Behavioral neuroscience.

[7]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Learning internal representations by error propagation , 1986 .

[8]  J. Lisman,et al.  Heightened synaptic plasticity of hippocampal CA1 neurons during a Cholinergically induced rhythmic state , 1993, Nature.

[9]  J. Theios,et al.  Acquisition and extinction of a classically conditioned response in hippocampectomized rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus). , 1972, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[10]  I. Izquierdo,et al.  Mechanism of action of scopolamine as an amnestic. , 1989, Trends in pharmacological sciences.

[11]  N. Mackintosh,et al.  Learned Irrelevance is not the Sum of Exposure to CS and US , 1995, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[12]  G. Buzsáki,et al.  Phase relations of hippocampal projection cells and interneurons to theta activity in the anesthetized rat , 1983, Brain Research.

[13]  A Ward,et al.  Pentoxifylline , 1987, Drugs.

[14]  Geoffrey E. Hinton Connectionist Learning Procedures , 1989, Artif. Intell..

[15]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  Modulation of the input/output function of rat piriform cortex pyramidal cells. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vol. 1: foundations , 1986 .

[17]  Gastone G. Celesia,et al.  A 30-Week Randomized Controlled Trial of High-Dose Tacrine in Patients With Alzheimer's Disease , 1994 .

[18]  D. Blozovski,et al.  Enhancement of passive avoidance learning through small doses of intra-amygdaloid physostigmine in the young rat. Its relation to the development of acetylcholinesterase. , 1988, Developmental psychobiology.

[19]  M. Gluck,et al.  Hippocampal mediation of stimulus representation: A computational theory , 1993, Hippocampus.

[20]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  GABAergic modulation of hippocampal population activity: sequence learning, place field development, and the phase precession effect. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  K. Krnjević,et al.  Electrophysiological and pharmacological characteristics of facilitation of hippocampal population spikes by stimulation of the medial septum , 1982, Neuroscience.

[22]  R. Rescorla,et al.  A theory of Pavlovian conditioning : Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement , 1972 .

[23]  R. Hirsh The hippocampus and contextual retrieval of information from memory: a theory. , 1974, Behavioral biology.

[24]  W. F. Prokasy,et al.  Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory. , 1972 .

[25]  F. Eckenstein,et al.  Cholinergic innervation of the cerebellum of rat, rabbit, cat, and monkey as revealed by choline acetyltransferase activity and immunohistochemistry , 1992, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[26]  C. Flaherty,et al.  Current Topics in Animal Learning : Brain, Emotion, and Cognition , 1991 .

[27]  P. Holland,et al.  Disruption of Decrements in Conditioned Stimulus Processing by Selective Removal of Hippocampal Cholinergic Input , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  Robert A. Jacobs,et al.  Increased rates of convergence through learning rate adaptation , 1987, Neural Networks.

[29]  Robert Bolles,et al.  Constraints on Learning: Limitations and Predispositions , 1974 .

[30]  Kurt Hornik,et al.  Neural networks and principal component analysis: Learning from examples without local minima , 1989, Neural Networks.

[31]  Z. Borhegyi,et al.  Postsynaptic targets of GABAergic hippocampal neurons in the medial septum-diagonal band of broca complex , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[32]  G. Buzsáki Two-stage model of memory trace formation: A role for “noisy” brain states , 1989, Neuroscience.

[33]  R. Yokel,et al.  Hippocampal Acetylcholine Increases During Eyeblink Conditioning in the Rabbit , 1996, Physiology & Behavior.

[34]  M. Gluck,et al.  Dissociation of hippocampal and entorhinal function in associative learning: A computational approach , 1995, Psychobiology.

[35]  Theodore W. Berger,et al.  Hippocampal substrate of classical conditioning , 1980 .

[36]  H PETSCHE,et al.  The significance of the rabbit's septum as a relay station between the midbrain and the hippocampus. II. The differential influence of drugs upon both the septal cell firing pattern and the hippocampus theta activity. , 1962, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[37]  J. Coyle,et al.  Effects of continuous infusion of cholinergic drugs on memory impairment in rats with basal forebrain lesions. , 1989, The Journal of pharmacology and experimental therapeutics.

[38]  Richard F. Thompson,et al.  Topics in learning and performance , 1972 .

[39]  D. McTavish,et al.  Erratum to: Tacrine: a review of its pharmacodynamic and pharmacokinetic properties, and therapeutic efficacy in Alzheimer’s disease , 1994, Drugs & aging.

[40]  L R Squire,et al.  Lesions of the hippocampal formation but not lesions of the fornix or the mammillary nuclei produce long-lasting memory impairment in monkeys , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[41]  K. Krnjević Acetylcholine Receptors in Vertebrate CNS , 1975 .

[42]  Richard F. Thompson The neurobiology of learning and memory. , 1986, Science.

[43]  Bernard Widrow,et al.  Adaptive switching circuits , 1988 .

[44]  H. Lal,et al.  Effects of anticholinergic drugs on learning and memory , 1983 .

[45]  O. Vinogradova,et al.  Acetylcholine, theta-rhythm and activity of hippocampal neurons in the rabbit—IV. Sensory stimulation , 1993, Neuroscience.

[46]  W. Levy,et al.  Partial quantification of the associative synaptic learning rule of the dentate gyrus , 1983, Neuroscience.

[47]  M. Gluck,et al.  Context, conditioning, and hippocampal rerepresentation in animal learning. , 1994, Behavioral neuroscience.

[48]  J. Disterhoft,et al.  Metrifonate Treatment Enhances Acquisition of Eyeblink Conditioning in Aging Rabbits , 1997, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[49]  J. Moore,et al.  Classical differential and operant conditioning in rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus) with septal lesions. , 1975, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[50]  M. Hasselmo Neuromodulation and cortical function: modeling the physiological basis of behavior , 1995, Behavioural Brain Research.

[51]  P. Solomon Role of the hippocampus in blocking and conditioned inhibition of the rabbit's nictitating membrane response. , 1977, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[52]  J. Coyle,et al.  Alzheimer's disease and senile dementia: loss of neurons in the basal forebrain. , 1982, Science.

[53]  N. Schmajuk,et al.  Stimulus configuration, classical conditioning, and hippocampal function. , 1992, Psychological review.

[54]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  Laminar selectivity of the cholinergic suppression of synaptic transmission in rat hippocampal region CA1: computational modeling and brain slice physiology , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[55]  W. Cutting Handbook of pharmacology , 1972 .

[56]  W. B. Orr,et al.  Hippocampectomy selectively disrupts discrimination reversal conditioning of the rabbit nictitating membrane response , 1983, Behavioural Brain Research.

[57]  N. Schmajuk,et al.  Latent inhibition of the rat eyeblink response: Effect of hippocampal aspiration lesions , 1994, Physiology & Behavior.

[58]  Joel L. Davis,et al.  An Introduction to Neural and Electronic Networks , 1995 .

[59]  Ralph R. Miller,et al.  Learned irrelevance exceeds the sum of CS-preexposure and US-preexposure deficits. , 1988 .

[60]  P. Solomon,et al.  Disruption of human eyeblink conditioning after central cholinergic blockade with scopolamine. , 1993, Behavioral neuroscience.

[61]  P. Solomon,et al.  The septohippocampal cholinergic system and classical conditioning of the rabbit's nictitating membrane response. , 1981, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[62]  M. J. Scavio,et al.  Posttraining effects of amphetamine, chlorpromazine, ketamine, and scopolamine on the acquisition and extinction of the rabbit's conditioned nictitating membrane response. , 1992, Behavioral neuroscience.

[63]  P. Solomon,et al.  Altered activity in the hippocampus is more detrimental to classical conditioning than removing the structure. , 1983, Science.

[64]  R. Nicoll,et al.  Voltage clamp analysis of cholinergic action in the hippocampus , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[65]  R. Lubow Latent inhibition. , 1973, Psychological bulletin.

[66]  D. Woodruff-Pak,et al.  Eyeblink classical conditioning in H.M.: delay and trace paradigms. , 1993, Behavioral neuroscience.

[67]  D. Powell,et al.  Intraseptal scopolamine has differential effects on Pavlovian eye blink and heart rate conditioning. , 1985, Behavioral neuroscience.

[68]  N. J. Mackintosh,et al.  Stimulus selection: Learning to ignore stimuli that predict no change in reinforcement. , 1973 .

[69]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  Cholinergic modulation of activity-dependent synaptic plasticity in the piriform cortex and associative memory function in a network biophysical simulation , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[70]  B. Campbell,et al.  Ontogeny of Adrenergic Arousal and Cholinergic Inhibitory Mechanisms in the Rat , 1969, Science.

[71]  G. Buzsáki,et al.  Hippocampal theta activity following selective lesion of the septal cholinergic systeM , 1994, Neuroscience.

[72]  W. Seifert Neurobiology of the hippocampus , 1983 .

[73]  T. Kaneko,et al.  Disruption of trace conditioning of the nictitating membrane response in rabbits by central cholinergic blockade , 1997, Psychopharmacology.

[74]  K. Davis,et al.  Enhancement of memory processes in Alzheimer's disease with multiple-dose intravenous physostigmine. , 1982, The American journal of psychiatry.

[75]  D. Salat,et al.  Intraseptal injection of the cholinergic immunotoxin 192-IgG saporin fails to disrupt latent inhibition in a conditioned taste aversion paradigm , 1996, Brain Research.

[76]  JOHN W. Moore,et al.  Central cholinergic blockade by scopolamine and habituation, classical conditioning, and latent inhibition of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response , 1976 .

[77]  S. D. Berry,et al.  Medial septal lesions retard classical conditioning of the nicitating membrane response in rabbits. , 1979, Science.

[78]  Y. Lamberty,et al.  Scopolamine disrupts visual reversal without affecting the first discrimination , 1987, Physiology & Behavior.

[79]  E. Warrington,et al.  Conditioning in amnesic patients , 1979, Neuropsychologia.

[80]  R. Kesner Reevaluation of the contribution of the basal forebrain cholinergic system to memory , 1988, Neurobiology of Aging.

[81]  A. G. Baker,et al.  Preexposure to the CS alone, US alone, or CS and US uncorrelated: Latent inhibition, blocking by context or learned irrelevance? , 1979 .

[82]  G. Davis Apomorphine and Other Dopaminomimetics: Basic Pharmacology, Vol. I , 1982 .

[83]  Mann Fc Tacrine therapy for the dementia of Alzheimer's disease. , 1994 .

[84]  M A Gluck,et al.  Late-training amnesic deficits in probabilistic category learning: a neurocomputational analysis. , 1996, Learning & memory.

[85]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  A Computational Model of Cholinergic Disruption of Septohippocampal Activity in Classical Eyeblink Conditioning , 1996, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[86]  M. M. Patterson,et al.  Fimbrial lesions and sensory preconditioning. , 1984, Behavioral neuroscience.

[87]  S. Sevush,et al.  Improved verbal learning after outpatient oral physostigmine therapy in patients with dementia of the Alzheimer type. , 1991, The Journal of clinical psychiatry.

[88]  I. Daum,et al.  Classical conditioning in patients with severe memory problems. , 1989, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[89]  D. Alkon,et al.  Hippocampal lesions impair memory of short-delay conditioned eye blink in rabbits. , 1989, Behavioral neuroscience.

[90]  P. Solomon,et al.  Latent inhibition and stimulus generalization of the classically conditioned nictitating membrane response in rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus) following dorsal hippocampal ablation. , 1975, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[91]  G. Lynch,et al.  The distribution of septal projections to the hippocampus of the rat , 1973, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[92]  L. Thal,et al.  Oral physostigmine and lecithin improve memory in Alzheimer disease , 1983, Annals of neurology.

[93]  W. L. Milligan,et al.  Orienting and classical conditioning in the rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus): Effects of septal area lesions , 1976, Physiology & Behavior.

[94]  J. Pearce,et al.  Hippocampal Lesions Attenuate Latent Inhibition and the Decline of the Orienting Response in Rats , 1987, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[95]  Scopolamine disruption of septo-hippocampal activity and classical conditioning. , 1989, Behavioral neuroscience.

[96]  M. Gluck,et al.  Intact delay-eyeblink classical conditioning in amnesia. , 1995, Behavioral neuroscience.

[97]  I. Gormezano,et al.  Effects of scopolamine and methylscopolamine on classical conditioning of the rabbit nictitating membrane response. , 1983, The Journal of pharmacology and experimental therapeutics.

[98]  O. Vinogradova,et al.  Acetylcholine, theta-rhythm and activity of hippocampal neurons in the rabbit—I. Spontaneous activity , 1993, Neuroscience.