A Normative Model of Attention: Modulation of Neural Response

When a sensory stimulus is encoded in a lossy fashion for ef- ficient transmission, there are necessarily tradeoffs between the represented fidelity of various aspects of the input pat- tern. In the model of attention presented here, a top-down signal informs the encoder of these tradeoffs. Given an en- semble of input patterns and tradeoff requirements, our sys- tem can learn to encode its inputs optimally. This general model is instantiated in a simple network: an autoencoder with a bottleneck, innervated by a top-down attentional sig- nal, trained using backpropagation. The only information the encoder receives concerning the semantics of the top-down attentional signal is from the optimization criterion, which penalizes the system more heavily for errors made near a simple attentional spotlight. The modulation of neural activ- ity learned by this model qualitatively matches that measured in animals during covert visual attention tasks.

[1]  P Kuyper,et al.  Triggered correlation. , 1968, IEEE transactions on bio-medical engineering.

[2]  J. Hyvärinen,et al.  Effects of attention on multiunit responses to vibration in the somatosensory regions of the monkey's brain. , 1982, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[3]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Shape Recognition and Illusory Conjunctions , 1985, IJCAI.

[4]  R. Desimone,et al.  Selective attention gates visual processing in the extrastriate cortex. , 1985, Science.

[5]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Learning representations by back-propagating errors , 1986, Nature.

[6]  Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al.  Network model of shape-from-shading: neural function arises from both receptive and projective fields , 1988, Nature.

[7]  William Bialek,et al.  Reading a Neural Code , 1991, NIPS.

[8]  Ralph Linsker,et al.  How to Generate Ordered Maps by Maximizing the Mutual Information between Input and Output Signals , 1989, Neural Computation.

[9]  Joseph J. Atick,et al.  Towards a Theory of Early Visual Processing , 1990, Neural Computation.

[10]  Joseph J. Atick,et al.  Predicting Ganglion and Simple Cell Receptive Field Organizations , 1991, Int. J. Neural Syst..

[11]  Ralph Linsker,et al.  Local Synaptic Learning Rules Suffice to Maximize Mutual Information in a Linear Network , 1992, Neural Computation.

[12]  Zhaoping Li,et al.  Understanding Retinal Color Coding from First Principles , 1992, Neural Computation.

[13]  D. V. van Essen,et al.  A neurobiological model of visual attention and invariant pattern recognition based on dynamic routing of information , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[14]  B. Motter Neural correlates of attentive selection for color or luminance in extrastriate area V4 , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[15]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[16]  Zhaoping Li,et al.  A Theory of the Visual Motion Coding in the Primary Visual Cortex , 1996, Neural Computation.

[17]  David J. Field,et al.  Emergence of simple-cell receptive field properties by learning a sparse code for natural images , 1996, Nature.

[18]  William Bialek,et al.  Spikes: Exploring the Neural Code , 1996 .

[19]  D. V. van Essen,et al.  Spatial Attention Effects in Macaque Area V4 , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of spatial selective attention in areas V1, V2, and V4 of macaque visual cortex. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  PIERRE VAN DE LAAR,et al.  Task-Dependent Learning of Attention , 1997, Neural Networks.

[22]  R. Desimone,et al.  Competitive Mechanisms Subserve Attention in Macaque Areas V2 and V4 , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  E. DeYoe,et al.  A physiological correlate of the 'spotlight' of visual attention , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[24]  Stefan Treue,et al.  Feature-based attention influences motion processing gain in macaque visual cortex , 1999, Nature.

[25]  Carrie J. McAdams,et al.  Effects of Attention on Orientation-Tuning Functions of Single Neurons in Macaque Cortical Area V4 , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  C. Gilbert,et al.  Attention and primary visual cortex. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[27]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  fMRI evidence for objects as the units of attentional selection , 1999, Nature.

[28]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The physiological basis of attentional modulation in extrastriate visual areas , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[29]  J. Maunsell,et al.  Effects of Attention on the Processing of Motion in Macaque Middle Temporal and Medial Superior Temporal Visual Cortical Areas , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  J. Maunsell,et al.  Attention to both space and feature modulates neuronal responses in macaque area V4. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  R. Knight,et al.  Prefrontal modulation of visual processing in humans , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[32]  E. Niebur,et al.  Growth patterns in the developing brain detected by using continuum mechanical tensor maps , 2022 .

[33]  S. Kakade,et al.  Learning and selective attention , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[34]  G. Mangun,et al.  The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[35]  R. Desimone,et al.  Attention Increases Sensitivity of V4 Neurons , 2000, Neuron.

[36]  R. Desimone,et al.  Modulation of Oscillatory Neuronal Synchronization by Selective Visual Attention , 2001, Science.

[37]  C. Koch,et al.  Computational modelling of visual attention , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[38]  Si Wu,et al.  Attention Modulation of Neural Tuning Through Peak and Base Rate , 2001, Neural Computation.

[39]  B McElree,et al.  Covert attention accelerates the rate of visual information processing , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[40]  A. U.S.,et al.  Predictability , Complexity , and Learning , 2002 .

[41]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[42]  F. Bremmer,et al.  Visual receptive field modulation in the lateral intraparietal area during attentive fixation and free gaze. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[43]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Attention, spatial representation, and visual neglect: simulating emergent attention and spatial memory in the selective attention for identification model (SAIM). , 2003, Psychological review.

[44]  Gustavo Deco,et al.  A Neurodynamical Model of Visual Attention: Feedback Enhancement of Spatial Resolution in a Hierarchical System , 2001, Journal of Computational Neuroscience.

[45]  Barak A. Pearlmutter,et al.  A normative model of attention: receptive field modulation , 2004, Neurocomputing.