Cortico-hippocampal interaction and adaptive stimulus representation: A neurocomputational theory of associative learning and memory

Computational models of the hippocampal region link psychological theories of associative learning with their underlying physiological and anatomical substrates. Our approach to theory development began with a broad description of the computations that depend on the hippocampal region in classical conditioning (Gluck and Myers, 1993 and Gluck and Myers, 2001). In this initial model, the hippocampal region was treated as an Information-processing system that transformed stimulus representations, compressing (making more similar) representations of inputs that co-occur or are otherwise redundant, while differentiating (or making less similar) representations of inputs that predict different future events. This model led to novel predictions for the behavioral consequences of hippocampal-region lesions in rodents and of brain damage in humans who have amnesia or are in the earliest stages of Alzheimer's disease. Many of these predictions have, since been confirmed by our lab and others. Functional brain imaging studies have provided further supporting evidence. In more recent computational modeling, we have shown how some aspects of this proposed information-processing function could emerge from known anatomical and physiological characteristics of the hippocampal region, including the entorhinal cortex and the septo-hippocampal cholinergic system. The modeling to date lays the groundwork for future directions that increase the depth of detail of the biological modeling, as well as the breadth of behavioral phenomena addressed. In particular, we are working now to reconcile these kinds of incremental associative learning models with other models of the hippocampal region that account for the rapid formation of declarative memories.

[1]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  How the Basal Ganglia Use Parallel Excitatory and Inhibitory Learning Pathways to Selectively Respond to Unexpected Rewarding Cues , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  K. Doya Complementary roles of basal ganglia and cerebellum in learning and motor control , 2000, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[3]  W. Schultz,et al.  Responses of monkey dopamine neurons to reward and conditioned stimuli during successive steps of learning a delayed response task , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[4]  M. Hasselmo Neuromodulation: acetylcholine and memory consolidation , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  Catherine E. Myers,et al.  Gateway to Memory: An Introduction to Neural Network Modeling of the Hippocampus and Learning , 2000 .

[6]  M. Gluck,et al.  l-dopa impairs learning, but spares generalization, in Parkinson's disease , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  Ralph R. Miller,et al.  Information processing in animals : memory mechanisms , 1983 .

[8]  M. Meeter,et al.  A computational approach to memory deficits in schizophrenia , 2002, Neurocomputing.

[9]  M. Gluck,et al.  Cortico-hippocampal representations in simultaneous odor discrimination: a computational interpretation of Eichenbaum, Mathews, and Cohen (1989). , 1996, Behavioral neuroscience.

[10]  M. Gluck,et al.  Hippocampal mediation of stimulus representation: A computational theory , 1993, Hippocampus.

[11]  W. Levy A computational approach to hippocampal function , 1989 .

[12]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Learning internal representations by error propagation , 1986 .

[13]  R. Desimone,et al.  The representation of stimulus familiarity in anterior inferior temporal cortex. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  M. Gluck,et al.  Context, conditioning, and hippocampal rerepresentation in animal learning. , 1994, Behavioral neuroscience.

[15]  J Ambros-Ingerson,et al.  Simulation of paleocortex performs hierarchical clustering. , 1990, Science.

[16]  L. Nadel,et al.  Multiple trace theory of human memory: Computational, neuroimaging, and neuropsychological results , 2000, Hippocampus.

[17]  M. Gluck,et al.  Integrating incremental learning and episodic memory models of the hippocampal region. , 2005, Psychological review.

[18]  K. Malmberg,et al.  Context-dependent recognition memory: the ICE theory. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[19]  M. Gluck,et al.  Integrating behavioral and physiological models of hippocampal function , 1996, Hippocampus.

[20]  Catherine E. Myers,et al.  Cerebellar Substrates for Error Correction in Motor Conditioning , 2001, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[21]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vol. 1: foundations , 1986 .

[22]  M. W. Brown,et al.  Differential neuronal encoding of novelty, familiarity and recency in regions of the anterior temporal lobe , 1998, Neuropharmacology.

[23]  G. Bower,et al.  From conditioning to category learning: an adaptive network model. , 1988 .

[24]  G. Mandler Recognizing: The judgment of previous occurrence. , 1980 .

[25]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Hippocampal conjunctive encoding, storage, and recall: Avoiding a trade‐off , 1994, Hippocampus.

[26]  M. Gluck,et al.  Dissociating medial temporal and basal ganglia memory systems with a latent learning task , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[27]  W. Scoville,et al.  LOSS OF RECENT MEMORY AFTER BILATERAL HIPPOCAMPAL LESIONS , 1957, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[28]  M Meeter,et al.  Mode shifting between storage and recall based on novelty detection in oscillating hippocampal circuits , 2004, Hippocampus.

[29]  Peter Dayan,et al.  A Neural Substrate of Prediction and Reward , 1997, Science.

[30]  M. Gluck,et al.  Dissociation of hippocampal and entorhinal function in associative learning: A computational approach , 1995, Psychobiology.

[31]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. , 1995, Psychological review.

[32]  Mark O West,et al.  Operant behavior during sessions of intravenous cocaine infusion is necessary and sufficient for phasic firing of single nucleus accumbens neurons , 1997, Brain Research.

[33]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  A model for experience-dependent changes in the responses of inferotemporal neurons , 2000, Network.

[34]  M. Gluck,et al.  Representation and Association in Memory: A Neurocomputational View of Hippocampal Function , 1995 .

[35]  M. Humphreys,et al.  Different Ways to Cue a Coherent Memory System: A Theory for Episodic, Semantic, and Procedural Tasks. , 1989 .

[36]  D Marr,et al.  Simple memory: a theory for archicortex. , 1971, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[37]  L. Nadel,et al.  The medial temporal region and memory consolidation: A new hypothesis , 2014 .

[38]  R. Rescorla A theory of pavlovian conditioning: The effectiveness of reinforcement and non-reinforcement , 1972 .

[39]  David S. Touretzky,et al.  Operant behavior suggests attentional gating of dopamine system inputs , 2001, Neurocomputing.

[40]  Catherine E. Myers,et al.  Further implications of a computational model of septohippocampal cholinergic modulation in eyeblink conditioning , 1998, Psychobiology.

[41]  G. Lynch,et al.  Activity of dentate granule cells during learning: Differentiation of perforant path input , 1979, Brain Research.

[42]  P. Suppes,et al.  Contemporary Developments in Mathematical Psychology , 1976 .

[43]  J. Theios,et al.  Acquisition and extinction of a classically conditioned response in hippocampectomized rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus). , 1972, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[44]  Martijn Meeter,et al.  Consolidation of long-term memory: evidence and alternatives. , 2004, Psychological bulletin.

[45]  N. Schmajuk,et al.  Occasion setting: Associative learning and cognition in animals. , 1998 .

[46]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  Laminar selectivity of the cholinergic suppression of synaptic transmission in rat hippocampal region CA1: computational modeling and brain slice physiology , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[47]  Richard C. Atkinson,et al.  Search and decision processes in recognition memory. , 1974 .

[48]  H. Eichenbaum The Hippocampal System and Declarative Memory in Animals , 1992, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[49]  M. Gluck,et al.  Probabilistic classification learning in amnesia. , 1994, Learning & memory.

[50]  Gwenn S. Smith,et al.  EARLY MARKER FOR ALZHEIMER'S DISEASE: THE ATROPHIC HIPPOCAMPUS , 1989, The Lancet.

[51]  M. Gluck,et al.  Interactive memory systems in the human brain , 2001, Nature.

[52]  Bryan J. Travis,et al.  A Computational Model of the Cerebellum , 1992 .

[53]  Mony J de Leon,et al.  Hippocampal Atrophy Disrupts Transfer Generalization in Nondemented Elderly , 2002, Journal of geriatric psychiatry and neurology.

[54]  R. O’Reilly,et al.  Conjunctive representations in learning and memory: principles of cortical and hippocampal function. , 2001, Psychological review.

[55]  E. Rolls,et al.  Modification of the responses of hippocampal neurons in the monkey during the learning of a conditional spatial response task , 1993, Hippocampus.

[56]  J. Disterhoft,et al.  Hippocampectomy disrupts trace eye-blink conditioning in rabbits. , 1990, Behavioral neuroscience.

[57]  L. Nadel,et al.  Memory consolidation, retrograde amnesia and the hippocampal complex , 1997, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[58]  Richard Granger,et al.  A cortical model of winner-take-all competition via lateral inhibition , 1992, Neural Networks.

[59]  M A Gluck,et al.  Impaired delay eyeblink classical conditioning in individuals with anterograde amnesia resulting from anterior communicating artery aneurysm rupture. , 2001, Behavioral neuroscience.

[60]  Mark A. Gluck,et al.  A dynamic model of learning in the septo-hippocampal system , 2000, Neurocomputing.

[61]  M A Gluck,et al.  Computational models of the neural bases of learning and memory. , 1993, Annual review of neuroscience.

[62]  Rafal Bogacz,et al.  The restricted influence of sparseness of coding on the capacity of familiarity discrimination networks , 2002, Network.

[63]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  Free recall and recognition in a network model of the hippocampus: simulating effects of scopolamine on human memory function , 1997, Behavioural Brain Research.

[64]  Brita Elvevåg,et al.  Reduced parahippocampal connectivity produces schizophrenia-like memory deficits in simulated neural circuits with reduced parahippocampal connectivity. , 2005, Archives of general psychiatry.

[65]  William B. Levy,et al.  Temporal Sequence Compression by an Integrate-and-Fire Model of Hippocampal Area CA3 , 2004, Journal of Computational Neuroscience.

[66]  Catherine E. Myers,et al.  Dissociating basal forebrain and medial temporal amnesic syndromes: Insights from classical conditioning , 2002, Integrative physiological and behavioral science : the official journal of the Pavlovian Society.

[67]  Mark A. Gluck,et al.  Selective entorhinal and nonselective cortical-hippocampal region lesions, but not selective hippocampal lesions, disrupt learned irrelevance in rabbit eyeblink conditioning , 2002, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[68]  W B Levy,et al.  A sequence predicting CA3 is a flexible associator that learns and uses context to solve hippocampal‐like tasks , 1996, Hippocampus.

[69]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  A Computational Model of Cholinergic Disruption of Septohippocampal Activity in Classical Eyeblink Conditioning , 1996, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[70]  Rafal Bogacz,et al.  Comparison of computational models of familiarity discrimination in the perirhinal cortex , 2003, Hippocampus.

[71]  W. F. Prokasy,et al.  Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory. , 1972 .

[72]  R. Lubow Latent inhibition. , 1973, Psychological bulletin.

[73]  P. Solomon,et al.  Latent inhibition and stimulus generalization of the classically conditioned nictitating membrane response in rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus) following dorsal hippocampal ablation. , 1975, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[74]  Kurt Hornik,et al.  Neural networks and principal component analysis: Learning from examples without local minima , 1989, Neural Networks.

[75]  M. Gluck,et al.  Ibotenic acid lesions of the medial septum retard delay eyeblink conditioning in rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus). , 2002, Behavioral neuroscience.

[76]  James Zackheim,et al.  A temporally sensitive recurrent network model of occasion setting. , 1998 .

[77]  G. Lynch,et al.  The neurobiology of learning and memory , 1989, Cognition.

[78]  Geoffrey E. Hinton Connectionist Learning Procedures , 1989, Artif. Intell..

[79]  M. Gluck,et al.  Dissociating entorhinal and hippocampal involvement in latent inhibition. , 2000, Behavioral neuroscience.

[80]  E. Bostock,et al.  Experience‐dependent modifications of hippocampal place cell firing , 1991, Hippocampus.

[81]  R. Passingham The hippocampus as a cognitive map J. O'Keefe & L. Nadel, Oxford University Press, Oxford (1978). 570 pp., £25.00 , 1979, Neuroscience.

[82]  H Eichenbaum,et al.  Further studies of hippocampal representation during odor discrimination learning. , 1989, Behavioral neuroscience.

[83]  M. Gluck,et al.  Intact delay-eyeblink classical conditioning in amnesia. , 1995, Behavioral neuroscience.

[84]  L. Squire Memory and Brain , 1987 .

[85]  M. Gluck,et al.  Dissociating Hippocampal versus Basal Ganglia Contributions to Learning and Transfer , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[86]  W. Levy,et al.  Partial quantification of the associative synaptic learning rule of the dentate gyrus , 1983, Neuroscience.

[87]  M A Gluck,et al.  Latent learning in medial temporal amnesia: evidence for disrupted representational but preserved attentional processes. , 2000, Neuropsychology.

[88]  M A Gluck,et al.  Late-training amnesic deficits in probabilistic category learning: a neurocomputational analysis. , 1996, Learning & memory.

[89]  Bernard Widrow,et al.  Adaptive switching circuits , 1988 .

[90]  D. Davies,et al.  MYOCARDIAL DISORDERS IN THE ELDERLY. , 1963, Lancet.

[91]  R. O’Reilly,et al.  Modeling hippocampal and neocortical contributions to recognition memory: a complementary-learning-systems approach. , 2003, Psychological review.

[92]  Joel L. Davis,et al.  An Introduction to Neural and Electronic Networks , 1995 .

[93]  E. Rolls,et al.  Computational analysis of the role of the hippocampus in memory , 1994, Hippocampus.

[94]  R. O’Reilly,et al.  Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.6 No.12 December 2002 , 2022 .

[95]  A. Yonelinas The Nature of Recollection and Familiarity: A Review of 30 Years of Research , 2002 .