ACh, Uncertainty, and Cortical Inference

Acetylcholine (ACh) has been implicated in a wide variety of tasks involving attentional processes and plasticity. Following extensive animal studies, it has previously been suggested that ACh reports on uncertainty and controls hippocampal, cortical and cortico-amygdalar plasticity. We extend this view and consider its effects on cortical representational inference, arguing that ACh controls the balance between bottom-up inference, influenced by input stimuli, and top-down inference, influenced by contextual information. We illustrate our proposal using a hierarchical hidden Markov model.

[1]  D. Mackay The Epistemological Problem for Automata , 1956 .

[2]  J. Pearce,et al.  A model for Pavlovian learning: Variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. , 1980 .

[3]  Lawrence R. Rabiner,et al.  A tutorial on hidden Markov models and selected applications in speech recognition , 1989, Proc. IEEE.

[4]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  Acetylcholine and memory , 1993, Trends in Neurosciences.

[5]  Gerald Sommer,et al.  Pattern Recognition by Self-Organizing Neural Networks , 1994 .

[6]  M. Hasselmo Neuromodulation and cortical function: modeling the physiological basis of behavior , 1995, Behavioural Brain Research.

[7]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  The Helmholtz Machine , 1995, Neural Computation.

[8]  David Mumford,et al.  Neuronal Architectures for Pattern-theoretic Problems , 1995 .

[9]  M. Sarter,et al.  Behavioral vigilance following infusions of 192 IgG-saporin into the basal forebrain: selectivity of the behavioral impairment and relation to cortical AChE-positive fiber density. , 1996, Behavioral neuroscience.

[10]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  Encoding and retrieval of episodic memories: Role of cholinergic and GABAergic modulation in the hippocampus , 1998, Hippocampus.

[11]  Peter Dayan,et al.  A Neural Substrate of Prediction and Reward , 1997, Science.

[12]  M. Sarter,et al.  Cognitive functions of cortical acetylcholine: toward a unifying hypothesis , 1997, Brain Research Reviews.

[13]  P. Holland Brain mechanisms for changes in processing of conditioned stimuli in Pavlovian conditioning: Implications for behavior theory , 1997 .

[14]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Generative models for discovering sparse distributed representations. , 1997, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[15]  Rajesh P. N. Rao,et al.  Dynamic Model of Visual Recognition Predicts Neural Response Properties in the Visual Cortex , 1997, Neural Computation.

[16]  T. Robbins,et al.  Central cholinergic systems and cognition. , 1997, Annual review of psychology.

[17]  Jean-Marc Fellous,et al.  Computational Models of Neuromodulation , 1998, Neural Computation.

[18]  P. Holland,et al.  Amygdala circuitry in attentional and representational processes , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[19]  H. Pflüger Neuromodulation during motor development and behavior , 1999, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[20]  M. Hasselmo Neuromodulation: acetylcholine and memory consolidation , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[21]  Peter Dylan Recurrent sampling models for the Helmholtz machine , 1999 .

[22]  K. Doya Metalearning, neuromodulation, and emotion , 2000 .

[23]  D. Heeger,et al.  Activity in primary visual cortex predicts performance in a visual detection task , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[24]  S. Kakade,et al.  Learning and selective attention , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[25]  Peter Dayan,et al.  Dopamine Bonuses , 2000, NIPS.

[26]  Peter Dayan,et al.  Acetylcholine in cortical inference , 2002, Neural Networks.